Sex differences in thermal nociception and morphine antinociception in rodents depend on genotype

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(00)00015-4Get rights and content

Abstract

It has been appreciated for some time that the sexes can differ in their sensitivity to pain and its inhibition. Both the human and rodent literatures remain quite contentious, with many investigators failing to observe sex differences that others document clearly. Recent data from our laboratory have pointed to an interaction between sex and genotype in rodents, such that sex differences are observed in some strains but not others. However, these studies employed inbred mouse strains and are thus not directly relevant to existing data. We presently examined whether the observation of statistically significant sex differences in nociception and morphine antinociception might depend on the particular outbred rodent population chosen for study. Rats of both sexes and three common outbred strains were obtained from three suppliers (Long Evans, Simonsen; Sprague Dawley, Harlan; Wistar Kyoto, Taconic) and tested for nociceptive sensitivity on the 49°C tail-withdrawal assay, and antinociception following morphine (1–10 mg/kg, i.p.). In further studies, three outbred populations of mice (CD-1, Harlan; Swiss Webster, Harlan; Swiss Webster, Simonsen) were bred in our vivarium for several generations and tested for tail-withdrawal sensitivity and morphine antinociception (1–20 mg/kg, i.p.). We observed all possible sex comparison patterns for these two traits in different rodent populations: male>female, female>male, and no significant difference. In a separate study in which the estrous cycle was tracked in female mice, we found evidence for an interaction between genotype and estrous phase relevant to morphine antinociception. However, estrous cyclicity did not explain the observed sex differences. These data are discussed with respect to the existing sex difference and pain literature, and also as they pertain to future investigations of these phenomena.

Introduction

It has long been suspected that men and women have different thresholds for and tolerance to painful stimuli (see [16], [35], [96] for reviews). In human populations, these differences are often attributed to the cognitive and sociocultural processes involved in labeling stimuli as painful and not to primary biological differences in pain processing between the sexes. Although it is difficult to eliminate these influences in laboratory tests of sex differences in human pain processing, it seems reasonable to expect that uncovering possible sex differences in the processing of nociceptive stimuli in animal subjects should be relatively straightforward. However, as in the human pain experimental literature, there exists a lack of consensus as to the existence and direction of sex differences in rodent subject populations.

Although the use of varying methodologies clearly contribute to the inconsistencies in laboratory studies, this factor cannot account for all of the contradictory reports. One interpretation of these conflicting findings is that sex differences in nociceptive processing do exist, but we have not yet identified all of the modulating variables that must be controlled for in experimental studies in order to clearly and consistently demonstrate them. Another possibility is that sex differences exist in some subject populations, but not in others (or that they exist in all populations, but to differing degrees). It is also likely that some experimental designs are simply not sufficiently powerful to detect existing differences, especially those of modest magnitude. Indeed, numerous reports indicate that although differences between males and females in nociceptive responses seemed apparent, these differences did not reach the criterion for statistical significance. The purpose of this investigation was to clarify the nature of the existing rodent literature and to attempt to reconcile inconsistencies, by demonstrating that the detection of sex differences is dependent on the particular subject population under study.

Section snippets

Sex differences in nociceptive sensitivity in rodents

Although the literature regarding human sex differences in pain has been the subject of a number of recent reviews and meta-analyses [16], [35], [85], [96], to our knowledge the analogous rodent literature has never been rigorously reviewed. Table 1summarizes the results of all investigations to our knowledge that have tested for, and reported the results of, basal nociceptive responses of both male and female rodents. Although some of these investigations measured sex differences in response

Sex differences in morphine antinociception in rodents

Unlike the human literature, which remains controversial (see Ref. [71]), the rodent literature on sex differences in opioid antinociception is relatively consistent (see Ref. [17] for review). Table 2presents the results of all investigations to our knowledge that have tested for, and reported the results of, morphine antinociception in both male and female rodents. Again, this list is likely to be incomplete for reasons described above. Virtually all studies report a sex difference with males

Methodological variability between studies

Of the various methodological differences between laboratories that can be invoked to help explain inconsistent findings—circadian rhythmicity, differences in algesiometric assay parameters (e.g. stimulus intensity), and hormonal fluctuations associated with estrous phase—none appears to obviously account for the observed variability. For example, although nociceptive sensitivity may vary between the sexes according to photoperiodicity, the vast majority of studies in both rats and mice have

Interaction of sex and genotype in nociception and antinociception

Recent research from our laboratory suggests that genetic differences accounting for inter-individual variability in pain-related behaviors may be dependent on sex. For example, we found a quantitative trait locus (QTL, a chromosomal region containing a gene or genes influencing a continuously distributed trait) on mouse chromosome 4 that was statistically associated with baseline hot-plate latency in male mice but not in females [78]. We also uncovered a female-specific QTL associated with

Animals

Although the genetically heterogeneous populations used in these studies are not “strains” in the technical sense, this term will nonetheless be used for simplicity. All studies were performed in accordance with standard guidelines for the care and use of laboratory animals, and were approved by the local animal use committee.

Rats

A two-way (strain, sex) ANOVA performed on baseline TW latency (averaged over three consecutive assessments) revealed a significant main effect of strain (F2,173=12.93,P<0.001) and a significant strain×sex interaction (F2,173=4.60,P<0.001). The main effect of sex did not approach significance (F1,173=0.39). One-way ANOVAs revealed significant main effects of strain in both male (F2,86=6.80,P<0.005) and female (F2,86=15.95,P<0.001) rats. Tukey's post-hoc test revealed that male SD rats displayed

Discussion

The present studies clearly demonstrate the influence of genotype on thermal nociceptive sensitivity and morphine antinociception. Further, these data illustrate that sex differences in these traits are dependent on the outbred rodent population chosen for study. That is, based on the outbred strain selected, sex differences on the TW test will be either readily observed, subtle, or non-existent. Even the direction of sex differences (i.e. male>female or female>male) is strain-dependent in

Acknowledgements

Supported by USPHS grants DA11394 and DE12735 to J.S.M. Thanks to Jagdeep Sodhi, Julie Markham, Anna Kokayeff and Camron D. Bryant for help with data collection, and to Brenda Edwards and her staff for excellent care of animals.

References (101)

  • J. Candido et al.

    Effect of adrenal and sex hormones on opioid analgesia and opioid receptor regulation

    Pharmacol Biochem Behav

    (1992)
  • I. Ceccarelli et al.

    Effects of novelty and pain on behavior and hippocampal extracellular ACh levels in male and female rats

    Brain Res

    (1999)
  • F.M. Clark et al.

    Anatomical evidence for genetic differences in the innervation of the rat spinal cord by noradrenergic locus coeruleus neurons

    Brain Res

    (1992)
  • R.M. Craft et al.

    Sex differences in discriminative stimulus and diuretic effects of the k opioid agonist U69,593 in the rat

    Pharmacol Biochem Behav

    (1998)
  • R.M. Craft et al.

    Sex differences in cocaine- and nicotine-induced antinociception in the rat

    Brain Res

    (1998)
  • A. d'Amore et al.

    High-intensity nociceptive stimuli minimize behavioral effects induced by restraining stress during the tail-flick test

    J Pharmacol Toxicol Meth

    (1992)
  • M. Dennis

    Sex-dependent and sex-independent neural control of reactivity to electric footshock in the rat

    Exp Neurol

    (1972)
  • R.A. Drury et al.

    Differential effects of ovarian hormones on reactivity to electric footshock in the rat

    Physiol Behav

    (1978)
  • R.G. Fessler et al.

    Variations in postweaning environment and sensitivity to electric shock in male and female rats

    Behav Biol

    (1976)
  • R.B. Fillingim et al.

    Gender differences in the responses to noxious stimuli

    Pain Forum

    (1995)
  • L.J. Forman et al.

    The response to analgesia testing is affected by gonadal steroids in the rat

    Life Sci

    (1989)
  • C.A. Frye et al.

    Hormonal milieu affects tailflick latency in female rats and may be attenuated by access to sucrose

    Physiol Behav

    (1992)
  • C.A. Frye et al.

    Diet and estrous cycle influence pain sensitivity in rats

    Pharmacol Biochem Behav

    (1993)
  • P. Gray et al.

    Stress-induced responses and open-field behavior in estrous and nonestrous mice

    Physiol Behav

    (1982)
  • R.B. Kanarek et al.

    Chronic running-wheel activity decreases sensitivity to morphine-induced analgesia in male and female rats

    Pharmacol Biochem Behav

    (1998)
  • B.G. Kasson et al.

    Endocrine influences on the actions of morphine. IV. Effects of sex and strain

    Life Sci

    (1984)
  • M. Kavaliers et al.

    Sex differences in N-methyl-D-aspartate involvement in k opioid and non-opioid predator-induced analgesia in mice

    Brain Res

    (1997)
  • M. Kavaliers et al.

    Sex differences in opioid and non-opioid mediated predator-induced analgesia in mice

    Brain Res

    (1991)
  • M. Kavaliers et al.

    Sex differences in the expression and antagonism of swim stress-induced analgesia in deer mice vary with the breeding season

    Pain

    (1995)
  • M. Kavaliers et al.

    Daily rhythms of analgesia in mice: effects of age and photoperiod

    Brain Res

    (1983)
  • M. Kavaliers et al.

    Sex and day–night differences in opiate-induced responses of insular wild deer mice, Peromyscus maniculatus triangularis

    Pharmacol Biochem Behav

    (1987)
  • M. Kavaliers et al.

    Stress-induced opioid analgesia and activity in deer mice: sex and population differences

    Brain Res

    (1987)
  • M. Kavaliers et al.

    Novelty-induced opioid analgesia in deer mice (Peromyscus maniculatus): sex and population differences

    Behav Neural Biol

    (1988)
  • M. Kavaliers et al.

    Developmental changes in opiate-induced analgesia in deer mice: sex and population differences

    Brain Res

    (1990)
  • M. Kavaliers et al.

    Sex differences in the effects of neuropeptide FF and IgG from neuropeptide FF on morphine- and stress-induced analgesia

    Peptides

    (1992)
  • M. Kavaliers et al.

    Sex differences in the effects of Tyr-MIF-1 on morphine- and stress-induced analgesia

    Peptides

    (1992)
  • M. Kavaliers et al.

    Sex differences in naloxone- and Tyr-MIF-1-induced hypoalgesia

    Peptides

    (1993)
  • M. Kavaliers et al.

    Sex differences in the antinociceptive effects of the enkephalinase inhibitor, SCH 34826

    Pharmacol Biochem Behav

    (1993)
  • V. Kayser et al.

    Estrous and sex variations in vocalization thresholds to hindpaw and tail pressure stimulation in the rat

    Brain Res

    (1996)
  • K.L. Kepler et al.

    Roles of gender, gonadectomy and estrous phase in the analgesic effects of intracerebroventricular morphine in rats

    Pharmacol Biochem Behav

    (1989)
  • K.L. Kepler et al.

    Gender effects and central opioid analgesia

    Pain

    (1991)
  • E.K. Krzanowska et al.

    Morphine antinociception elicited from the ventrolateral periaqueductal gray is sensitive to sex and gonadectomy differences in rats

    Brain Res

    (1999)
  • M.N. Leer et al.

    Elevated shock threshold in sexually receptive female rats

    Physiol Behav

    (1988)
  • S.M. Lipa et al.

    Sex differences in the inhibitory effects of the NMDA antagonist, MK-801, on morphine and stress-induced analgesia

    Brain Res Bull

    (1990)
  • H.E. Marks et al.

    Changes in stimulus reactivity following gonadectomy in male and female rats of different ages

    Physiol Behav

    (1972)
  • M. Martinez-Gomez et al.

    Assessing pain threshold in the rat: changes with estrus and time of day

    Physiol Behav

    (1994)
  • L. Menendez et al.

    Gender and test dependence of a type of kappa mediated stress-induced analgesia in mice

    Gen Pharmac

    (1994)
  • C. Miaskowski et al.

    Does opioid analgesia show a gender preference for females?

    Pain Forum

    (1999)
  • J.S. Mogil et al.

    Sex and genotype determine the selective activation of neurochemically-distinct mechanisms of swim stress-induced analgesia

    Pharmacol Biochem Behav

    (1997)
  • J.S. Mogil et al.

    Transgenic studies of pain

    Pain

    (1998)
  • Cited by (299)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text