INTRODUCTION

Tobacco use is the single most preventable cause of morbidity and mortality in the United States (CDC, 2010). Among the ingredients of tobacco, nicotine is the principal psychoactive agent (Le Foll and Goldberg, 2006, 2009). Nicotine is unique among the many addictive substances in that, despite the high rate of relapse (80%) (Hajek et al, 2009), its primary reinforcing effect is weak (Stolerman and Jarvis, 1995). For example, smoking produces minimal euphoric sensation (Walker et al, 2001). Other subjective effects of nicotine, such as clear-headedness, feelings of relief, fatigue recovery, or hyperactivity, are all mild (Kono et al, 2001). Sensory cues associated with nicotine delivery have a critical role in nicotine addiction (Palmatier et al, 2007). For example, operant lever presses were synergistically increased after combining nicotine with an audiovisual stimulus (Donny et al, 2003). Over 400 flavor additives are used in various tobacco products (Baker et al, 2004). These olfactogustatory (OG) cues associated with nicotine delivery enhance the addictiveness of nicotine. For example, blocking the OG cues reduced the hedonic rating of smoking, as well as puff self-administration (SA) (Perkins et al, 2001), while smokers preferring menthol-flavored cigarettes are nearly twice as likely to relapse as are non-menthol smokers (Pletcher et al, 2006).

In addition to its weak reinforcing effect, the addictive liability of nicotine is even more perplexing when one considers the fact that most people experience negative subjective effects during their initial exposure of tobacco, such as coughing, dizziness, and nausea (Chen et al, 2003; DiFranza et al, 2004). The aversive effect of nicotine is also evident from animal studies. For example, rats received nicotine injections after consuming flavored liquid reliably avoided that flavor in subsequent tests (Korkosz et al, 2006; Laviolette et al, 2002; Rinker et al, 2008; Shram et al, 2006, Wilmouth and Spear, 2004) (ie, conditioned taste aversion, CTA). Despite the aversive effects, many people continue to smoke and become addicted. The majority (80%) of smokers initiated their consumption of tobacco products in social settings (Greenlund et al, 1997). Peer smoking is one of the strongest predictors of smoking initiation (Greenlund et al, 1997). A study following >700 twins for 16 years found that smoking behavior of friends, rather than genetics, explained the greater concordance for smoking among adult monozygotic pairs than among dizygotic pairs. Most interestingly, social support (encourage vs discourage) during the first puff also discriminated those who became smokers and those who did not (Hofstetter et al, 2007). Although these findings demonstrated the critical roles of social interaction on human smoking behavior, the majority of preclinical research of smoking has precluded the study of social interaction by using isolated animals.

We report an animal model of nicotine SA in social settings. In this model, i.v. nicotine was delivered with a contingent OG cue. Social transmission of the OG cue from a demonstrator rat permitted the acquisition of nicotine SA. This model provides a unique opportunity to study interactions between nicotine, OG stimulation, and social behavior.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Drugs

Nicotine hydrogen tartrate was purchased from Sigma (St Louis, MO). Nicotine dosages (pH=7.2; calculated as free base), freshly prepared in saline for each cohort of animals, were calculated to deliver 15–60 μg/kg body weight in a 50 μl volume. Heparin and Methohexital were purchased from Buttler Schein Animal Health (Dublin, OH).

Animals

Adolescent Sprague–Dawley rats (postnatal days, (PN) 29–31) (Harlan Laboratories, Madison, WI) were given 7 days to acclimation to a reversed 12 : 12 h light–dark cycle (lights off at 0930 hours). All procedures were conducted in accordance with the NIH Guidelines Concerning the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and were approved by the Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of Tennessee.

Nicotine SA

Jugular catheters constructed from PE-60 and silastic tubing were implanted in adolescent Sprague–Dawley rats between PN 36 and 38 as previously described (Chen et al, 2007). Rats received 3 days of post surgery recovery. All SA sessions were conducted in the dark-phase of the light cycle and lasted 3 h. Standard rat chow and water were provided ad libitum throughout the experiments, but neither was available during the SA sessions. All rats were group housed with 2–4 peers receiving the same treatment to avoid social isolation induced stress.

Two drinking spouts, each connected to one lickometer controller, were fitted on the same wall of the operant chambers (Med Associates, St Albans, VT). A small current is past between the wire mesh floor and the drinking spout on licking, allowing the number and timing of licks to be recorded. A white cue light and a tone generator were located on the same wall as the drinking spouts. SA was conducted using a fixed ratio 10 and 20-s time-out schedule. Thus, 10 licks on the active spout triggered the concurrent delivery of 60 μl OG cue to the spout and 50 μl of nicotine (or saline) through the jugular catheter. The OG cue was 0.4% saccharin solution containing 0.5% Hershey's unsweetened cocoa (or 0.1% unsweetened grape Kool-Aid). A mixture of glucose (3%) and saccharin (0.125%) (Smith et al, 1976) was also tested in one experiment. The cue light and tone were also turned on during the infusion. Licking on the inactive spout had no programmed consequence. The patency of the jugular catheters was tested periodically using Methohexital (0.2 ml, 10 mg/ml).

Progressive ratio testing was conducted on day 11 of SA in one experiment. The number of licks to obtain subsequent infusion was determined using the exponential formula (5 exp (0.2 × injection number)−5), such that the required responses per injection were as follows: 3, 6, 10, 15, 20, 25, 32, 40, 50, 62, 77, 95, 118, 145, 179, 219, 268, 328, 402, 492, and 603. Break points were defined by >20 min of inactivity on the active spout (Shram et al, 2008).

One group of adolescent female rats received yoked nicotine injections (15 μg/kg) while self-administering the OG solution using FR10 schedule. Both the number and the timing of nicotine injections were programmed according to data obtained from solo rats self-administering nicotine at 15 μg/kg.

Social Interaction During Nicotine SA

A divider located in the middle of the operant chamber separated two rats located on each side, but allowed orofacial contact. A randomly selected demonstrator rat (always the same sex as the SA rat) had free access to the same OG cue received by the SA rat but did not receive i.v. injections. On average, demonstrator rats consumed 30–40 ml of the OG cue per session. The SA rat had access to dedicated drinking spouts as described.

Effect of Familiarity with Demonstrator Rats on Nicotine SA

Adolescent Lewis rats were used as demonstrators for Sprague–Dawley nicotine SA rats. The SA rats were housed with either the Lewis demonstrators (familiar) or other Sprague–Dawley rats age-matched to the Lewis rats (thus, unfamiliar with the demonstrators) for 1 week before jugular catheter implantation. In the familiar group, the pairing of nicotine SA rats to the Lewis demonstrators was fixed, while a random Lewis demonstrator rat was selected before each session for the SA rats in the unfamiliar group.

Statistics

Data were presented as mean±SEM. All lick and injection data were analyzed by repeated-measures ANOVA, with spout and day treated as within-subject variables. Post hoc analysis was conducted using the Tukey HSD procedure. In dependent t-tests were also applied as noted in the results section. All statistical analyses were conducted using R statistical package (http://www.r-project.org).

RESULTS

Adolescent Rats Did Not Self-Administer i.v. Nicotine with Contingent OG Cues

Adolescent rats (all females) implanted with jugular catheters were subjected to daily 3-h nicotine SA sessions starting on PN 41–43 in the absence of a demonstrator rat. Licking on the active spout that met reinforcement schedule (FR10, time out 20 s) triggers the concurrent delivery of i.v. nicotine (15 or 30 μg/kg) or saline and 60 μl 20 mM saccharin solution containing 0.5% unsweetened cocoa. For rats receiving i.v. saline (n=5, Figure 1a), there were significant effect for spout (active vs inactive: F1,40=44.0, p<0.001) on the number of licks. An average of 161.6±90.4 licks was emitted on the active spout on day 1, escalating to 4162.0±2016.8 licks on day 10 (effect of day on active licks: F9,36=2.8, p<0.05). The effect of day on the number of inactive licks was not significant (F9,36=2.1, p>0.05). Thus, the OG cue was appetitive when paired with i.v. saline injection.

Figure 1
figure 1

Adolescent rats did not self-administer i.v. nicotine with contingent OG cues. Adolescent female rats (postnatal days 41–43) were subjected to daily 3-h nicotine SA sessions. Rats licking on the active spout meeting reinforcement schedule (fixed ratio 10 with 20-s time out) received either 15, 30 μg/kg nicotine, or saline through a jugular catheter and 60 μl OG cue (20 mM saccharin, 0.5% unsweetened cocoa) through the active spout. Licking on the inactive spout had no programmed consequences. Rats receiving i.v. saline (a) significantly increased the number of active licks over 10 sessions (p<0.001). An average of 161.6±90.4 active licks was recorded on day 1, escalating to 4162.0±2016.8 licks on day 10. Licks on the active spout were significantly higher than on the inactive spout (p<0.001). Rats receiving 15 μg/kg (b) or 30 μg/kg (c) nicotine injections paired with the OG cue did not significantly change the number of licks over the 10 sessions (p>0.05). The number of licks was not statistically different between the two spouts for rats receiving 15 μg/kg (p>0.05) but was significantly lower on the active spout for rats receiving 30 μg/kg (p<0.01). The total numbers of active licks (d) and injections (e) over the 10 sessions were significantly lower in i.v. nicotine rats (ps<0.001). (f) Rats received yoked nicotine (15 μg/kg) gradually increased the number of active licks (p<0.001) and developed preference for the active spout (p<0.001), demonstrating that contingent delivery of nicotine and OG cue is required for the reduction in active licks. Together, these results suggest that adolescent rats do not self-administer nicotine with contingent OG cue, likely because of conditioned taste aversion. ***p<0.001 compared with rats self-administering i.v. saline.

PowerPoint slide

In rats receiving 15 μg/kg nicotine injections (n=6, Figure 1b), there was no significant effect of day (F9,45=1.0, p>0.05) or spout (F1,50=2.4, p>0.05) on the number of licks. Rats receiving 30 μg/kg nicotine injections (n=7, Figure 1c) showed no significant change on the number of licks by day (F9,54=1.1, p>0.05). The number of licks were significantly different between the spouts (F1,60=7.2, p<0.01), with fewer licks emitted on the active spout during days 1–5. Comparing the number of active licks from the three treatments (i.v. saline, 15 and 30 μg/kg nicotine, Figure 1d) found a significant main effect (F2,15=27.4, p<0.001). Post hoc analysis found the number of licks was significantly different between all three treatments (p<0.001 for saline vs the two nicotine groups and p<0.01 between the nicotine groups). The number of injections was also significantly different between the three treatments (F2,15=26.4, p<0.001 for the main effect, p<0.001 for all pairwise comparisons). Another group of rats (n=5, Figure 1f) received programmed i.v. nicotine (15 μg/kg) injections while self-administering the OG solution using a FR10 schedule. These yoked rats showed clear preference for saccharine (effect of day: F9,36=14.0, p<0.001; effect of spout: F1,40=49.3, p<0.001). Therefore, contingent pairing of nicotine with OG cue was necessary to reduce active licks. Together, these results showed that adolescence female rats do not self-administer nicotine with contingent OG cue, even when the OG cue was inherently appetitive. This is potentially because the formation of CTA.

To further test whether CTA was the cause for the reduction in licking when nicotine was available, one group of adolescent female rats (n=6, Figure 2a) received OG solution using FR10 schedule for 13 days, with nicotine (30 μg/kg) available on days 1–3 and 7–9. No injection was given on days 4–6 and 10–12. Nicotine was available for 1 h on day 13. The number of active licks progressively declined during the first 3 nicotine days (effect of day: F2,10=6.3, p<0.05) but rapidly increased on days 4–6 (effect of day: F2,10=7.3, p<0.05), when nicotine was not available. The number of active licks was significantly lower on day 4 when compared with rats received i.v. saline (cf. Figure 1a, p<0.05, independent t-test). In addition, the number of active licks on day 6 was significantly higher than those on days 4 and 5 (p<0.05, Tukey HSD post hoc). These data suggested that CTA was acquired on days 1–3 and was extinguished during days 4–6. The availability of nicotine on days 7–9 reduced active licks (p<0.05, day 6 vs 7; p<0.01, day 6 vs days 8 and 9, Tukey HSD post hoc), confirming the aversive effect of nicotine. The number of active licks emitted on day 6 is not statistically different from those emitted during the second extinction phase (days 10–12, p>0.05, Tukey HSD), suggesting extinction was likely complete on day 6. The large number of active licks on day 10 further suggested that the additional pairings of nicotine with the OG cue on days 7–9 did not strengthen CTA and the reduction in active licks during days 7–9 was likely due to the acute aversive effect of nicotine. This was tested on day 13 by making nicotine available only during the second hour of the 3-h session. Rats licked throughout the 3-h sessions on day 12, when nicotine was not delivered (Figure 2b). On day 13, rats licked similar amount compared with day 12 during the first hour (Figure 2c, F1,=6.2, p>0.05, repeated-measures ANOVA) but significantly reduced licking once nicotine was delivered (Figure 2c, F1,4=12.7, p<0.05. The number of active licks was 19.1%, further reduced to 0.8% for the first and last 10 min, respectively, when compared with the same time period on day 12). Licking resumed once nicotine delivery was stopped during the last hour (F1,4=2.44, p>0.05, compared with day 12). Together, these data suggested that the aversive effect of nicotine was acute but it forms conditioned aversion once its delivery was paired with an OG cue.

Figure 2
figure 2

Conditioned taste aversion induced by self-administered nicotine. Adolescent female rats were subjected to 13 daily 3-h SA sessions with the OG cue. Nicotine (30 μg/kg) was delivered on days 1–3, 7–9 for the entire session and on day 13 for 1 h. (a) The number of active licks on day 4 was lower (p<0.05) than those emitted by rats received i.v. saline (cf. Figure 1a), demonstrating CTA. More licks emitted on day 6 after additional extinction training. Licking was significantly reduced on day 7 when nicotine was available, suggesting the aversive effect of nicotine potentially was acute. After additional extinction trainings, rats licked throughout the 3-h sessions on day 12, when nicotine was not delivered (b). However, when nicotine was made available during the second hour of next session, licking behavior was immediately reduced (c). Withholding nicotine delivery again restored licking. When comparing day 12 vs day 13, the number of licks were different for the second hour (p<0.05) but not for the first and third hour. Together, these data suggested that the aversive effect of self-administered nicotine was detected acutely by rats and formed CTA when paired with OG cue. *p<0.05 compared with day 6. #p<0.01 compared with the second hour on day 12.

PowerPoint slide

Social Interaction Permitted Nicotine SA with OG Cue

As social interaction is known to modulate preference for novel food (Galef, 1986; Galef et al, 1997), we exposed each SA rat (females) to a demonstrator rat (females) during the SA sessions in which the SA rat had free access to the OG cue. In the presence of the demonstrators, rats receiving saline injection showed strong preference for the active over the inactive spout (n=6, Figure 3a, F1,50=248.1, p<0.001). Rats receiving 15 μg/kg nicotine (n=6, Figure 3b) gradually escalated the number of active licks to 1413.2±172.1 on the last session (27.6±7.4-fold increase compared with the first session; the effect of day on the number of licks: F9,45=6.2, p<0.001). As a result, rats received 28.3±5.2 nicotine injections on the last session (7.1±2.1-fold increase compared with the first session; the effect of day on the number of injections: F9,45=6.6, p<0.001). The active spout was strongly preferred over the inactive spout (F1,50=127.1, p<0.001. The ratio of active/inactive licks was 351.8±122.4 on last session). Rats receiving 30 μg/kg nicotine injection (n=6, Figure 3c) also demonstrated strong preference for the active spout (F1,50=28.0, p<0.001. The ratio of active/inactive licks was 47.1±19.2). An average of 603.0±139.6 licks was emitted on the active spout, which resulted in 14.2±2.8 nicotine injections on the last day of SA. The effect of day on the number of licks (F9,45=3.4, p<0.01) and nicotine injections (F9,45=4.0, p<0.001) were also statistically significant. In contrast, rats receiving 60 μg/kg nicotine (n=5, Figure 3d) emitted significantly more licks on the inactive spout than on the active spout (F1,40=6.8, p<0.05). The effect of day on the number of licks was not significant (F9,36=0.6, p>0.05). An alternative olfactory cue (unsweetened grape Kool-Aid) was also tested to confirm that the acquisition behavior is not specific to the OG cue that included unsweetened cocoa (n=5, Figure 3e). The number of active licks was not statistically significant between OG cue with cocoa and OG cue with Kool-Aid at the same nicotine dose (F1,9=0.04, p>0.05); neither was the number of injections different (F1,9=1.3, p>0.05). The amount of nicotine self-administered at different doses during days 7–10 is shown in Figure 3f. There was a significant effect of nicotine dose (F2,14=9.2, p<0.01). Post hoc Tukey HSD test found that rats took significantly less (p<0.01) nicotine at 60 μg/kg than they did at the two lower doses.

Figure 3
figure 3

Social interaction promoted nicotine SA with OG cue. The operant chambers were divided by a center panel with holes that separated the rats but allowed physical contact (see Materials and methods section). During SA, each SA rat was exposed to a demonstrator rat, which had free access to the OG cue and received no nicotine injection. Rats self-administering saline (a) injections showed strong preference for the active over the inactive spout (p<0.001). Rats receiving 15 μg/kg (b) and 30 μg/kg (c) nicotine gradually increased the number of active licks (effect of day: p<0.001 and p<0.01, respectively) and showed strong preference for the active spout (p<0.001 for both doses). In contrast, rats receiving 60 μg/kg nicotine (d) licked more on the inactive spout than on the active spout (p<0.05). The effect of day on the number of licks was not significant (p>0.05). An alternative olfactory cue (unsweetened grape Kool-Aid) (e) was tested to confirm that acquisition behavior was not specific to the cocoa used in the OG cue. At 30 μg/kg nicotine, the number of active licks was not statistically significant between OG cue with cocoa or OG cue with Kool-Aid (p>0.05); neither was the number of injections different (p>0.05). The average amount of nicotine self-administered at different doses during the days 7–10 is shown in (f). The was a significant effect of nicotine dose (p<0.01). Post hoc Tukey HSD test found that rats took significantly less (p<0.01) nicotine at 60 μg/kg than at the two lower doses. *p<0.01 compared with rats self-administering 15 or 30 μg/kg nicotine.

PowerPoint slide

Nicotine SA Is Not the Result of Enhanced Appetitiveness of the OG Cue

As the presence of demonstrators tend to increase the number of active licks in rats receiving i.v. saline (F=4.7, p=0.059, cf. Figures 1a and 3a), we tested whether increasing the appetitiveness of the OG cue, by using a mixture of saccharin and glucose (Smith et al, 1976), was sufficient to support nicotine SA. Solo female adolescent rats receiving i.v. saline with contingent oral delivery of saccharin glucose mixture (Smith et al, 1976) emitted a similar amount of licks as rats receiving i.v. saline with saccharine/cocoa in the presence of demonstrators (Figure 4, F1,9=0.03, p>0.05). In a progressive ratio test, the break points achieved by these two treatments were not statistically different (Figure 4b, t-test, p>0.05). However, when saccharine glucose mixture was contingently delivered with nicotine without demonstrators, nicotine SA was not established (n=6, Figure 4c. Effect of day on number of licks and injections: F9,45=1.5, p>0.05 and F9,45=1.1, p>0.05, respectively; effect of spout on licks: F1,5=2.6, p>0.05). Therefore, increasing the appetitiveness of the contingent OG cue alone was not sufficient to support nicotine SA.

Figure 4
figure 4

Increasing the appetitiveness of the OG cue was not sufficient to support nicotine SA. Rats are known to have strong preference to saccharine+glucose mixture. The number of active licks emitted by solo rats receiving i.v. saline paired with saccharine+glucose mixture (a) was not statistically different from the number of active licks emitted by rats receiving OG cue with the demonstrators (p>0.05, cf. Figure 2b). Progressive ratio test (b) found that the break points achieved by these two treatments were not statistically different (p>0.05). However, when saccharine+glucose mixture was used as the OG cue in the absence of demonstrators (c), stable nicotine intake was not established. The p-value for the effect of day on licks and injections, and the difference between spouts are all >0.05.

PowerPoint slide

Contribution of Nicotine to Operant Licking Behavior

To confirm that nicotine was reinforcing the operant licking behavior, the OG cue was removed after stable SA had been established. The average number of active licks and injections for the last 3 days of SA with OG cue and subsequence 3 days of SA without the OG cue were shown (Figure 5). Licking was reduced by 98.9% in saline rats after OG cue removal (n=5, Figure 5a, F1,18=29.9, p<0.01). The number of injections was reduced by 97.8% (Figure 5b, F1,18=28.1, p<0.001), indicating in rats receiving saline injection, licking behavior is solely driven by the OG solution. In rats that self-administered 30 μg/kg nicotine (and receiving vehicle s.c. injection before each session), the number of active licks was reduced by 46.9% after the removal of OG cue (n=7, Figure 5a, F1,26=7.8, p<0.05). However, the number of injections was reduced by only 8.1% in the absence of the OG cue (Figure 5b, F1,26=0.4, p>0.05), indicating licking behavior is driven, in part, by nicotine in these animals. Licking by nicotine SA rats was reduced by 84.4% (n=5, Figure 5a F1,18=14.4, p<0.01) and the number of injections was reduced by 72.4% (Figure 5b, F1,18=16.7, p<0.001), when mecamylamine, a non-competitive nicotinic receptor antagonist (1.5 mg/kg, s.c.) was administered 15 min before each of the SA sessions in which the OG cue was absent, further confirming the role of nicotine in operant licking behavior. Compared with nicotine SA rats receiving vehicle injections, rats receiving mecamylamine injections had significantly fewer active licks (F1,10=9.8, p<0.05) and obtained significantly fewer injections (F1,10=7.5, p<0.01). Therefore, in the absence of the OG cue, nicotine still reinforced the licking behavior.

Figure 5
figure 5

Operant licking behavior was partially reinforced by nicotine. OG cue was removed once stable SA was established. The average number of active licks (a) and injections (b) for the 3 days before and after removal of the OG cue was shown. In control rats receiving saline injections, removing the OG cue reduced active licks and injections by 98.9% and 97.8%, respectively (p<0.001 for both), indicating licking behavior was entirely driven by the OC due in these rats. For rats self-administering 30 μg/kg nicotine, the number of active licks was reduced by 46.9% (p<0.05) after the removal of OG cue, indicating licking behavior was only partially driven by the OG cue in these animals. The role of nicotine in reinforcing licking behavior is further supported by the insignificant reduction (8.1%, p>0.05) in nicotine injection in the absence of OG cue. Licking on the active spout however, was significantly reduced (84.4%, p<0.01) when mecamylamine, a non-competitive nicotinic receptor antagonist was administered (1.5 mg/kg, s.c.) 15 min before each of the OG cue-lacking SA sessions, so was the number of injections significantly reduced (72.4%, p<0.001). **p<0.01, ***p<0.001 compare with days with OG cue. #p<0.05; ##p<0.01, compared with vehicle injection before SA.

PowerPoint slide

Social Information Is Encoded in Olfaction

To identify the social information that permitted the acquisition of nicotine SA, we removed the olfactory component (unsweetened cocoa) from the OG cue of both the demonstrator rats and the SA rats (all females). Nicotine SA was not established under this condition (n=5, Figure 6a). The effects of day on active licks and injections were not significant (F9,36=1.5, p>0.05, and F9,36=1.3, p>0.05, respectively). The difference between active and inactive licks was not significant either (F1,4=0.4, p>0.05). In fact, the numbers of active licks were not statistically different from those tested in the absence of demonstrator rats (Figure 6b, F1,10=0.8, p>0.05). Thus, olfactory cue present in the demonstrator's breath is likely the source of the social information that modulated nicotine intake.

Figure 6
figure 6

Social information is encoded in olfaction. (a) Unsweetened cocoa, the olfactory component of the OG cue, was removed for both the demonstrator and SA rats. Nicotine SA was not established under this condition. The effects of day on active licks and injections were not significant (p>0.05 for both). The difference between active and inactive licks was not significant either (p>0.05). (b) The numbers of active licks and nicotine injections were not statistically different from those tested in the absence of demonstrators (p>0.05 for both).

PowerPoint slide

Social Interaction Is Not Required to Maintain Nicotine SA

Female adolescent rats self-administered 15 μg/kg nicotine for 10 sessions with demonstrators, followed by five sessions without the demonstrators (n=7, Figure 7). The numbers of active licks and nicotine injections were reduced by 24.6±6.9% and 25.0±8.1%, respectively, in the absence of the demonstrators (F1,54=20.5, p<0.001, and F1,54=18.4, p<0.001, respectively). However, rats still licked 1532.2±207.8 times on the active spouts without the demonstrators, which was 27.6±44.8-fold of those on the inactive spouts (effect of spout: F1,60=126.0, p<0.001). Therefore, although social interaction was not required to maintain nicotine SA with OG cue, it continued to promote nicotine intake.

Figure 7
figure 7

Social interaction was not required to maintain nicotine SA. Rats self-administered 15 μg/kg nicotine for 10 days with demonstrator rats. The demonstrators were absent for the five subsequent sessions. Rats maintained nicotine intake in the absence of the demonstrators. Although the numbers of active licks and nicotine injections were reduced by 24.6±6.9% and by 25.0±8.1%, respectively (p<0.01 and p<0.05, respectively), rats maintained a strong preference to the active spout in that the ratio of active vs inactive licks was 27.6±44.8 (active vs inactive: p<0.001).

PowerPoint slide

Familiar Peers Were More Effective in Facilitating Nicotine SA

Female Lewis rats were used as demonstrators for Sprague–Dawley SA rats to enlarge the difference in familiarity between the two treatment groups. Adolescent female Sprague–Dawley SA rats were housed with either the Lewis demonstrator rats (familiar group) or age-matched Sprague–Dawley rats (unfamiliar to the Lewis demonstrator rats) for 1 week before jugular surgery. Both groups acquired nicotine SA (Figure 8). The number of active licks and nicotine injections during the last 5 sessions by rats familiar to the demonstrators (previous cage-mates) were 2.6±0.3- and 1.7±0.1-fold greater than those unfamiliar to the demonstrator rats (F1,12=10.8, p<0.01 and F1,12=3.9, p=0.07), respectively. Thus, the capacity of familiar peers in modulating nicotine intake is greater than those of the unfamiliar peers.

Figure 8
figure 8

Familiar peer was more effective in promoting nicotine SA. Lewis rats were used as demonstrators. SD rats were housed either with the Lewis demonstrators for 1 week (a) or with other SD rats (b) before nicotine SA sessions. Both groups acquired nicotine SA (active vs inactive: p>0.01 for both). Rats exposed to familiar demonstrators licked 1283.6±193.3 times on the active spout during the last 5 days of SA. In contrast, rats exposed to unfamiliar demonstrators licked 504±86.5 times during the same time period, reflecting a 2.6±0.3-fold difference (p<0.01), which resulted in a 1.7±0.1-fold difference in nicotine intake (p=0.07).

PowerPoint slide

Effect of Sex on Acquisition

Male adolescent rats were subjected to nicotine (30 μg/kg) SA with contingent OG cue in the presence of male demonstrator rats. The number of active licks and nicotine injections significantly increased over the 10 daily sessions (n=7, Figure 9, F9,54=6.5, p<0.001, and F9,54=9.6, p<0.001, respectively). The number of licks was significantly different between the two spouts (F1,60=26.2, p<0.001). When compared with female adolescent rats subjected to the same treatment, neither the number of active licks (F1,11=3.1, p>0.05) nor the number of injections (F1,11=0.7, p>0.05) was statistically significant.

Figure 9
figure 9

No sex differences on acquisition. Male adolescent rats were subjected to nicotine (30 μg/kg) SA with contingent OG cue with male demonstrators. Both active licks and nicotine injections significantly increased over the 10 daily sessions (p<0.001 for both). Active licks were significantly higher than inactive licks (p<0.001). When compared with female adolescent rats subjected to the same treatment, neither the number of active licks nor the number of injections was statistically different (p>0.05 for both).

PowerPoint slide

DISCUSSION

We report an adolescent rat model of i.v. nicotine SA with contingent OG cue, in which social interaction was required for the acquisition, but not the maintenance of nicotine SA. It is generally recognized that drugs of abuse, including nicotine (Garrett and Griffiths, 2001; Harvey et al, 2004), have both rewarding and aversive effects. For example, rats have been shown to avoid saccharin solution if it is provided immediately before SA (Wheeler et al, 2008; Wise et al, 1976). Although continued SA demonstrated the rewarding effects, the avoidance of saccharin intake suggested that aversive effects were induced (for an alternative interpretation, see Grigson, 2008). We found that rats avoided the OG cue when it was paired with nicotine (15 or 30 μg/kg, Figures 1b and c), although the OG cue was appetitive to rats receiving i.v. saline (Figure 1a). A recent study, similar to ours, found that rats pressing a lever to receive contingent i.v. nicotine and an olfactory cue (through implanted oral catheter), failed to self-administer nicotine (Grebenstein and Rowland, 2010). These data are in agreement with reports demonstrating that nicotine induces CTA (Korkosz et al, 2006; Rinker et al, 2008; Wilmouth and Spear, 2004). Indeed, when rats received yoked nicotine, strong preference to the active spout was developed (Figure 1f), suggesting that contingent delivery of nicotine and the OG cue is critical for the reduction in licking. To test if CTA was acquired, one group of rats received OG cue for 13 days but nicotine was withheld on days 4–6 and 10–12 (Figure 2). As expected, active licks increased progressively during days 4–6, reaching levels similar to those receiving saline by day 6 (Figure 1a). Thus, the low amount of active licks on day 4 (4% of day 6) is likely a result of CTA. Neophobia, a potential alternative explanation, is unlikely because rats received i.v. saline (and should also experience neophobia) emitted >1000 active licks on day 4 (Figure 1a). When these rats again received nicotine on days 7–9, the number of licks reduced again. Together with the yoke data, the reduction on day 7 suggested that the aversive effect of nicotine is likely acute. This was confirmed by data showing that in rats received extinction training, strong licking response in the absence of nicotine was immediately reduced once nicotine was delivered, while withholding nicotine delivery again enabled licking (Figures 2b and c). These results indicating aversive effect as a result of nicotine exposure are consistent with reports in human subjects, in which negative effects (such as tension, jitteriness, and dysphoria) were reported not only on initial exposure (Chen et al, 2003; DiFranza et al, 2004; Shram et al, 2006) but also in experienced smokers after extended tobacco use (Garrett and Griffiths, 2001; Jones et al, 1999; Le Foll and Goldberg, 2009).

Although social environments, ranging from having friends who smoke (Flay et al, 1994) to being exposed to smoking scenes in movies (Charlesworth and Glantz, 2005; Sargent, 2005), have been shown to promote smoking in teenagers, peer interaction in rodents has also been shown to overcome CTA. For example, rats reduced subsequent intake of unfamiliar food if they were injected with illness-inducing agents after their initial exposure. However, exposing rats to a demonstrator who just consumed the same flavored food (Galef, 1986) or fluid (Galef et al, 1997) reversed this aversion. In fact, social transmission of food preference (STFP), that is, favoring an unfamiliar food after interacting with a conspecific immediately after it consumed the same food, is well established (Dindo et al, 2011; Galef et al, 1997; Solomon et al, 2002; Wrenn, 2004). Carbon disulfide, a volatile compound contained in rat breath and detected by dedicated olfactory sensory neurons (Munger et al, 2010), is required for STFP (Galef et al, 1988). We thus modeled social interaction in teenage smokers by including a demonstrator rat in the SA sessions. The demonstrator had free access to the OG cue but did not receive nicotine infusions. A divider with holes separated the demonstrator from the SA rat, but allowed orofacial interaction. In the presence of the demonstrator, stable nicotine SA was observed at both 15 and 30 μg/kg, but not at 60 μg/kg (Figure 3). The failure to establish nicotine SA at the highest dose was likely due to larger aversive effects of nicotine and the limited efficacy of social interaction (Galef and Whiskin, 1998). Once nicotine SA was acquired, social interaction was no longer required to maintain the behavior. However, social interaction still promoted nicotine intake after acquisition, because less nicotine was taken in the absence of the demonstrator (Figure 7).

As the OG cue was appetitive, it could be argued that SA was entirely driven by the OG cue. To test the role of nicotine in the operant licking behavior, we withheld the OG cue after 10 days of SA (Figure 5). Although nicotine SA rats reduced licking on the active spout by 46.9%, the number of injections they received was reduced by only 8.1%. The reduction in active licks reflected the portion of active licks emitted while consuming the OG cue. However, in the absence of the OG cue, rats continued to lick the dry spout to obtain nicotine. Application of mecamylamine, a non-competitive nicotinic acetylcholine receptor antagonist, significantly reduced nicotine intake by 72.4%, demonstrating that the operant licking behavior depends, in part, on nicotine. Although the OG cue makes considerable contribution toward licking behavior, it is possible that with prolonged training, the licking response will be more depend on nicotine rather than the OG cue, as demonstrated in non-human primate studies (Le Foll et al, 2007).

Odors are critical for social interaction in rodents. Removing unsweetened cocoa from the cue abolished nicotine SA (Figure 6a), while unsweetened Kool-Aid was equally effective as cocoa in supporting nicotine SA (Figure 3f). These findings showed that modulating nicotine intake by social interaction requires the social transmission of a nicotine-associated odor from a peer, but that the innate quality of the odor does not have a critical role.

We further tested the hypothesis that different social relations (ie, familiar vs unfamiliar) vary in their capacity to modulate nicotine intake. Figure 8 shows that rats accompanied by familiar peers had greater nicotine intake than those with unfamiliar peers. These results are in agreement with report that partner familiarity regulates voluntary ethanol intake in rats (Maldonado et al, 2008). Together, they indicate that the effect of social interaction on drug intake is not solely determined by a signature odor, such as carbon disulfide (Galef et al, 1988); rather, it also involves the complex process of individual recognition.

We used primarily female adolescent rats in this study. However, male adolescent rats also acquired nicotine SA at similar levels as females (Figure 9). Although this did not rule out the possibility that potential sex difference exists at different doses of nicotine and/or OG odor or reinforce schedule other than those studied (Chaudhri et al, 2005), nevertheless, social modulation of nicotine SA is not specific to females.

It is possible that demonstrators enhanced the appetitiveness of the OG cue, overcoming the aversiveness induced by nicotine and permitted nicotine SA. Thus, a sufficiently appetitive OG cue would support the acquisition of nicotine SA without social interaction. We tested this hypothesis by using a saccharin–glucose mixture, which is strongly preferred by rats (Smith et al, 1976). The appetitiveness and motivational effects of the saccharin–glucose mixture in the absence of the demonstrators were similar to those of saccharin in the presence of the demonstrators (Figures 3b and 4a). However, rats failed to acquire nicotine SA when saccharin–glucose mixture was the OG cue in the absence of demonstrators (Figure 4c). Therefore, enhancing the appetitiveness of the OG cue does not appear to be the mechanism by which social interaction modulates nicotine SA. Alternatively, social interaction might support nicotine SA with OG cue by enhancing the rewarding effects of nicotine. A recent study showed that a low dose of nicotine interacted with social reward to enhance conditioned place preference (Thiel et al, 2009). However, in our model, interaction with a demonstrator rat taking the saccharin solution without the odor cue (unsweetened cocoa) was not sufficient to support nicotine SA (Figure 6a). Thus, the enhancement of nicotine reward by social interaction, if present in our model, is not sufficient to support nicotine SA.

Based on forgoing discussion, the most likely mechanism for social interaction to enable nicotine SA in this model is by either blocking or reversing nicotine-induced CTA. The neurochemical or molecular mechanisms underlie this effect are unknown. Oxytocin, a neuropeptide synthesized by magnocellular neurons in hypothalasmus and supraoptic nucleus, has been implicated in numerous social behaviors, including parenting, affiliation, social cognition (Choleris et al, 2009; Insel, 2010; Ross and Young, 2009), and social recognition (Choleris et al, 2009). Oxytocin also regulates dopaminergic neurotransmission in the mesolimbic system (Melis and Argiolas, 2011). Therefore, oxytocin is a potential common molecular target that facilitates nicotine SA in rodents as well as in adolescent smokers.

In summary, we have established an i.v. nicotine SA model in which nicotine was delivered with a contingent OG cue in adolescent rats. Social interaction with a peer, actively consuming the same OG cue, was required to establish nicotine intake. Although social interaction was not required to maintain nicotine intake, it nevertheless promoted greater intake of nicotine once the behavior was established. This rodent model encompassed a comprehensive set of sensory and social cues implicated in human smoking behavior; therefore, it is an ideal tool to dissect the complex interplay between social and environmental factors and sensory stimulation, and the roles they have in determining the affective response to nicotine and the motivation to obtain nicotine. Our current findings support the postulate that adolescent social interactions may promote human smoking behavior by surmounting the aversive properties of nicotine.