Skip to main content
Advertisement

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current Issue
    • Fast Forward
    • Latest Articles
    • Archive
  • Information
    • Instructions to Authors
    • Submit a Manuscript
    • FAQs
    • For Subscribers
    • Terms & Conditions of Use
    • Permissions
  • Editorial Board
  • Alerts
    • Alerts
    • RSS Feeds
  • Virtual Issues
  • Feedback
  • Submit
  • Other Publications
    • Drug Metabolism and Disposition
    • Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics
    • Molecular Pharmacology
    • Pharmacological Reviews
    • Pharmacology Research & Perspectives
    • ASPET

User menu

  • My alerts
  • Log in
  • Log out
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Pharmacological Reviews
  • Other Publications
    • Drug Metabolism and Disposition
    • Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics
    • Molecular Pharmacology
    • Pharmacological Reviews
    • Pharmacology Research & Perspectives
    • ASPET
  • My alerts
  • Log in
  • Log out
  • My Cart
Pharmacological Reviews

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current Issue
    • Fast Forward
    • Latest Articles
    • Archive
  • Information
    • Instructions to Authors
    • Submit a Manuscript
    • FAQs
    • For Subscribers
    • Terms & Conditions of Use
    • Permissions
  • Editorial Board
  • Alerts
    • Alerts
    • RSS Feeds
  • Virtual Issues
  • Feedback
  • Submit
  • Visit Pharm Rev on Facebook
  • Follow Pharm Rev on Twitter
  • Follow ASPET on LinkedIn
Review ArticleReview Article

Animal Models of Drug Relapse and Craving after Voluntary Abstinence: A Review

Ida Fredriksson, Marco Venniro, David J. Reiner, Jonathan J. Chow, Jennifer M. Bossert and Yavin Shaham
Michael Nader, ASSOCIATE EDITOR
Pharmacological Reviews July 2021, 73 (3) 1050-1083; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1124/pharmrev.120.000191
Ida Fredriksson
Behavioral Neuroscience Branch, Intramural Research Program, National Institute on Drug Abuse, National Institute of Health, Baltimore, Maryland (I.F., M.V., D.J.R., J.J.C., J.M.B., Y.S.), and Center for Social and Affective Neuroscience, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Marco Venniro
Behavioral Neuroscience Branch, Intramural Research Program, National Institute on Drug Abuse, National Institute of Health, Baltimore, Maryland (I.F., M.V., D.J.R., J.J.C., J.M.B., Y.S.), and Center for Social and Affective Neuroscience, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
David J. Reiner
Behavioral Neuroscience Branch, Intramural Research Program, National Institute on Drug Abuse, National Institute of Health, Baltimore, Maryland (I.F., M.V., D.J.R., J.J.C., J.M.B., Y.S.), and Center for Social and Affective Neuroscience, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Jonathan J. Chow
Behavioral Neuroscience Branch, Intramural Research Program, National Institute on Drug Abuse, National Institute of Health, Baltimore, Maryland (I.F., M.V., D.J.R., J.J.C., J.M.B., Y.S.), and Center for Social and Affective Neuroscience, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Jennifer M. Bossert
Behavioral Neuroscience Branch, Intramural Research Program, National Institute on Drug Abuse, National Institute of Health, Baltimore, Maryland (I.F., M.V., D.J.R., J.J.C., J.M.B., Y.S.), and Center for Social and Affective Neuroscience, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Yavin Shaham
Behavioral Neuroscience Branch, Intramural Research Program, National Institute on Drug Abuse, National Institute of Health, Baltimore, Maryland (I.F., M.V., D.J.R., J.J.C., J.M.B., Y.S.), and Center for Social and Affective Neuroscience, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Michael Nader
Roles: ASSOCIATE EDITOR
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Relapse to drug use during abstinence is a defining feature of addiction. During the last several decades, this clinical scenario has been studied at the preclinical level using classic relapse/reinstatement models in which drug seeking is assessed after experimenter-imposed home-cage forced abstinence or extinction of the drug-reinforced responding in the self-administration chambers. To date, however, results from studies using rat relapse/reinstatement models have yet to result in Food and Drug Administration–approved medications for relapse prevention. The reasons for this state of affairs are complex and multifaceted, but one potential reason is that, in humans, abstinence is often self-imposed or voluntary and occurs either because the negative consequences of drug use outweigh the drug’s rewarding effects or because of the availability of nondrug alternative rewards that are chosen over the drug. Based on these considerations, we and others have recently developed rat models of relapse after voluntary abstinence, achieved either by introducing adverse consequences to drug taking (punishment) or seeking (electric barrier) or by providing mutually exclusive choices between the self-administered drug and nondrug rewards (palatable food or social interaction). In this review, we provide an overview of these translationally relevant relapse models and discuss recent neuropharmacological findings from studies using these models. We also discuss sex as a biological variable, future directions, and clinical implications of results from relapse studies using voluntary abstinence models. Our main conclusion is that the neuropharmacological mechanisms controlling relapse to drug seeking after voluntary abstinence are often different from the mechanisms controlling relapse after home-cage forced abstinence or reinstatement after extinction.

Significance Statement This review describes recently developed rat models of relapse after voluntary abstinence, achieved either by introducing adverse consequences to drug taking or seeking or by providing mutually exclusive choices between the self-administered drug and nondrug rewards. This review discusses recent neuropharmacological findings from studies using these models and discusses future directions and clinical implications.

I. Introduction

A main problem for treatment of drug addiction is relapse (see Table 1 for glossary of terms) to drug use after periods of abstinence (Wikler, 1973; O’Brien et al., 1992; Sinha, 2011). In humans, relapse and craving are commonly triggered by re-exposure to the drug itself, re-exposure to cues and contexts previously associated with drug use, or exposure to stressors (Wikler, 1973; O’Brien et al., 1992; Sinha, 2011). Over the last several decades, drug relapse and craving have typically been studied at the preclinical level using animal models in which laboratory mice, rats, or monkeys are trained to self-administer a drug and resumption of drug seeking (relapse) is assessed after experimenter-imposed extinction of the drug-reinforced responding in the self-administration chambers or forced abstinence in the home cage (Shaham et al., 2003; Bossert et al., 2013; Venniro et al., 2016).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
TABLE 1

Glossary of terms

Studies using the reinstatement model have shown that, after extinction of the drug-reinforced responding, drug seeking is reinstated after exposure to drug-priming injections (Stewart and de Wit, 1987), drug-associated discrete cues (See, 2002), contexts (Crombag et al., 2008), and different stressors (Mantsch et al., 2016). Studies using the home-cage forced-abstinence procedure have shown that across several drug classes, relapse to drug seeking in the presence of drug-associated cues and contexts progressively increases over time (Pickens et al., 2011; Wolf, 2016). This behavioral phenomenon is termed incubation of drug craving (Grimm et al., 2001) (Table 2). Recent translational studies reported that incubation of drug craving also occurs in humans (Bedi et al., 2011; Wang et al., 2013; Li et al., 2015a; Parvaz et al., 2016).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
TABLE 2

Incubation of drug craving: behavioral measures and learning processes

Over the years, many studies have shown good postdictive validity of the classic reinstatement and forced-abstinence models: Food and Drug Administration (FDA)-approved medications for opioid, nicotine, and alcohol addiction decrease relapse or reinstatement in these models (Epstein et al., 2006; Sinha et al., 2011; Heilig et al., 2016). However, results from studies using these models have yet to result in FDA-approved medications for relapse prevention or demonstrate true predictive validity (Reiner et al., 2019; Venniro et al., 2020a). Specifically, many translational clinical studies on the efficacy of potential pharmacological treatments identified in the reinstatement model were mostly negative [e.g., corticotropin-releasing factor receptor antagonists (Kwako et al., 2015; Schwandt et al., 2016), 5-HT2c (the 2c receptor subtype of 5-hydroxytryptamine) receptor agonists (Brandt et al., 2020), n-acetylcysteine (LaRowe et al., 2013), buspirone (Winhusen et al., 2014), α1 receptor antagonists (Simpson et al., 2018)] or showed moderate effects [e.g., α2 adrenoceptor agonists (Kowalczyk et al., 2015), see Venniro et al. (2020a)].

The reasons for this state of affairs are complex and multifaceted, but one possibility is potential lack of homology between the classic relapse/reinstatement models and the conditions that often lead to drug abstinence in humans. Specifically, in humans, abstinence is often self-imposed or voluntary despite drug availability and occurs either because the negative or adverse consequences (e.g., fear of incarceration, losing family and friends, securing money to obtain drugs) of drug use outweigh the drug’s rewarding effect or because of the availability of nondrug alternative rewards (e.g., family, friends, employment) that are chosen over the addictive drug (Marlatt, 1996; Epstein and Preston, 2003). Therefore, one potential limitation of the classic relapse/reinstatement models is that the focus has been exclusively on forced abstinence (i.e., experimenter-imposed), which presumably models instances such as inpatient care or a drug-free prison environment. These procedures, however, do not model voluntary abstinence, which is presumably very common in humans who use drugs (Heyman, 2013).

To more closely mimic human relapse that occurs after self-imposed or voluntary abstinence, we and others have recently developed animal models of drug relapse and craving after voluntary abstinence, achieved either by introducing negative consequences to ongoing drug self-administration or by introducing alternative nondrug rewards using discrete-choice procedures (Fig. 1). These relapse models are different from the classic models in which drug is not available and laboratory animals are forced to abstain from the drug prior to the reinstatement/relapse tests (Shaham et al., 2003). These newer models include punishment- or electric barrier–induced voluntary abstinence relapse models (Panlilio et al., 2003; Cooper et al., 2007; Marchant et al., 2013a; Fredriksson et al., 2020) and palatable food– or social choice–induced voluntary abstinence relapse models (Caprioli et al., 2015a; Venniro et al., 2018). The goal of these new models is to increase the translation potential (or the predictive validity) of animal relapse models and to advance our understanding of behavioral and neurobiological factors involved in drug relapse.

Fig. 1
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 1

Rodent models of voluntary abstinence. (A) Models of voluntary abstinence induced by adverse consequences. Schematic representation of voluntary abstinence achieved by either punishment [left panel—lever presses result in contingent drug delivery plus contingent foot shock (typically for 0.5 seconds) delivered throughout the entire metal grid] or electric barrier (middle panel—noncontingent shock is delivered throughout the session to the grid floor area near the drug-associated lever). Under these conditions, drug taking and seeking (right panel—red line) decreases by increasing the intensity of the foot shock. (B) Voluntary abstinence induced by providing alternative nondrug rewards. Schematic representation of voluntary abstinence achieved via mutually exclusive choice between natural rewards (palatable food—left panel; social interaction—middle panel) and drugs. Under these conditions, rats prefer the natural reward (right panel—blue line) over drugs (right panel—red line).

In this review, we first describe the experimental procedures used in the voluntary abstinence–based relapse models. We then discuss behavioral and neuropharmacological findings from studies using these models. We conclude by discussing future directions, clinical implications, and the role of sex in relapse after voluntary abstinence. In Table 1 we provide a glossary of terms (italic font in the text).

II. Relapse after Abstinence Induced by Adverse Consequences of Drug Taking and Seeking

A. Punishment-Induced Voluntary Abstinence Model

1. Overview

The procedure includes three phases: drug self-administration training, punishment-induced voluntary abstinence, and relapse tests. [Some of the studies described below compared the effect of drug priming or context exposure on resumption of operant responding after punishment vs. extinction. To differentiate between the two operations, we use the term relapse to describe resumption of responding after punishment (or choice) and the term reinstatement to describe resumption of responding after extinction.] During the training phase, laboratory animals are trained to self-administer a drug, and each drug delivery is temporally paired with a discrete cue. Next, during the punishment-induced abstinence phase, drug-taking behavior is suppressed by response-contingent foot shock in either the self-administration context or in a different context (Marchant et al., 2013b). During testing, which is typically performed 1 day after termination of punishment, relapse is assessed without shock. Relapse to drug seeking after punishment can also be tested after different abstinence periods in a manner analogous to spontaneous recovery after extinction (Shaham et al., 1997). In the punishment-induced abstinence model, the negative consequences are associated with drug taking (Krasnova et al., 2014; Marchant et al., 2019), and the model attempts to mimic abstinence in humans due to negative consequences of drug taking (e.g., losing family, friends, and employment). Relapse after punishment-induced abstinence has been studied in rats trained to self-administer remifentanil (a potent short-acting opioid agonist), cocaine, and methamphetamine (intravenously) and alcohol (orally delivered) (Table 3).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
TABLE 3

Relapse after punishment-induced voluntary abstinence: summary of main findings

2. Review of Studies

a. Drug-priming–induced relapse

Panlilio et al. (2003) reported that priming injections of remifentanil accelerated reacquisition of remifentanil self-administration after punishment-induced abstinence in male rats. In another study, the same authors compared the effect of drug priming on relapse after punishment versus extinction in male rats. They reported that the effect of priming injections of the benzodiazepine lorazepam, but not heroin, is dependent on the abstinence condition. Specifically, priming injections of heroin or the benzodiazepine lorazepam induced relapse to remifentanil seeking after punishment (Panlilio et al., 2005). In contrast, priming injections of heroin but not lorazepam induced reinstatement of remifentanil seeking after extinction. We discuss the clinical implications of these findings in section IV. Conclusions and Clinical Implications below.

b. Context-induced relapse
i. Alcohol Studies

Marchant et al. (2013a) modified the ABA context-induced reinstatement [renewal (Bouton and Bolles, 1979)] procedure (Crombag and Shaham, 2002) to study context-induced relapse after punishment. In this procedure, rats are trained to self-administer a drug in context A. Next, abstinence is achieved in context B by probabilistic response-contingent foot-shock punishment. Context B is distinct from context A in its tactile, visual, auditory, and circadian (time of day) features. The rats are then tested for relapse in context A and B in the absence of foot shock or drug.

In the first study, Marchant et al. (2013a) used male alcohol-preferring rats (Penn et al., 1978) and compared context-induced relapse after punishment versus extinction in rats trained to self-administer oral alcohol in context A. Rats were first exposed to an intermittent-access alcohol-intake procedure (free choice between alcohol and water) in the home cage three to four times per week (Wise, 1973). The rats were then trained to self-administer alcohol in context A. Next, all rats continued to self-administer alcohol in a different context (context B). For one group, 50% of alcohol-reinforced responses were punished by mild foot shock; two other groups either received noncontingent shocks or no shock. A fourth group was given extinction training in context B. Rats were then tested for relapse to alcohol seeking under extinction conditions in contexts A and B. Marchant et al. (2013a) reported reliable context A–induced relapse to alcohol seeking after punishment that was similar in magnitude to context A–induced reinstatement after extinction. This renewal effect was specific to the punishment (contingent shock) manipulation and did not occur after unpaired (noncontingent) shock exposure in context B, which did not suppress responding. The context-specific ABA renewal effect of punishment was also observed with cocaine and food rewards (Bouton and Schepers, 2015; Pelloux et al., 2018a).

In an initial mechanistic study, Marchant et al. (2014) used alcohol-preferring male rats to investigate the role of lateral hypothalamus (LH) because of its role in context-induced reinstatement after extinction. The authors found that context-induced relapse after punishment is associated with increased activity of LH [assessed by the activity marker Fos (Morgan and Curran, 1991)] and that reversible inactivation of LH using the GABAA+B receptor agonists muscimol + baclofen (McFarland and Kalivas, 2001) decreases relapse (Marchant et al., 2014). In this study, Marchant et al. (2014) also used Fos labeling and the retrograde tracer cholera toxin B (CTb) to determine projection-specific activation of projections to LH during the relapse tests. They found that context-induced relapse is associated with selective activation of nucleus accumbens (NAc) shell and ventral bed nucleus of stria terminalis (BNST) neurons projecting to LH but not projections from ventral and dorsal medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), lateral septum, and dorsal BNST (Marchant et al., 2014). The causal role of these projections in context-induced relapse after punishment is unknown.

In a follow-up study, the same authors used similar experimental methods to investigate the role of ventral subiculum (vSub) and its glutamatergic projection to NAc shell because of their roles in context-induced reinstatement after extinction (Bossert and Stern, 2014; Bossert et al., 2016). They found that in alcohol-preferring male rats, context-induced relapse after punishment is associated with increased vSub activity (assessed by Fos) and that muscimol + baclofen reversible inactivation of vSub decreases relapse (Marchant et al., 2016). Additionally, context-induced relapse was associated with selective activation of vSub projections to NAc shell but not projections from ventral mPFC, paraventricular thalamus (PVT), and basolateral amygdala (BLA). Finally, Marchant et al. (2016) used a chemogenetic retro-DREADD dual-virus approach (Rothermel et al., 2013) and reported that inhibition of the vSub to NAc shell projection decreases context-induced relapse. In a follow-up study on the role of NAc shell in context-induced relapse after punishment in alcohol-preferring male rats, Marchant and Kaganovsky (2015) reported that intracranial injections of the dopamine D1 receptor (Drd1) antagonist SCH 23390 into NAc shell or core decrease relapse. NAc shell activity is dependent on glutamate and dopamine Drd1-mediated neurotransmission (O’Donnell, 2003). Thus, a potential account of the results of the two studies is that context-induced relapse after punishment is dependent on synergetic activation of glutamatergic projections from vSub (Groenewegen et al., 1987) and dopaminergic projections from ventral tegmental area (VTA) (Nauta et al., 1978).

Finally, Campbell et al. (2018) reported that in alcohol-preferring male rats, paternal alcohol exposure led to decreased resistance to punishment in context B and decreased context-induced relapse after punishment in context A in male F1 (filial 1) offspring. The reasons for this unexpected “protective” effect of paternal alcohol exposure on alcohol seeking in male F1 offspring are unknown.

ii. Cocaine Studies

In two studies, Pelloux and colleagues investigated mechanisms of context-induced relapse to cocaine seeking after punishment-induced abstinence in male rats. In the first study, Pelloux et al. (2018a) used Fos to investigate brain regions activated during context-induced relapse. The authors found that relapse was associated with selective activation of dorsal and ventral mPFC, BLA, vSub, PVT, LH, anterior insula (AI), dorsolateral striatum (DLS) and dorsomedial striatum (DMS), lateral habenula, substantia nigra, and dorsal raphe but not NAc core and shell, central amygdala nucleus (CeA), LH, VTA, orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), lateral and medial septum, ventral pallidum (VP), ventral BNST, medial habenula, and median raphe (Pelloux et al., 2018a). However, the results of this correlational study should be interpreted with caution because it is unknown whether Fos expression is the cause or the consequence of drug relapse (Bossert et al., 2011).

In a second study, Pelloux et al. (2018b) used muscimol + baclofen inactivation to investigate the causal role of BLA and CeA in context-induced relapse after punishment in male rats. The authors compared the role of the amygdala subregions in context-induced relapse after punishment versus context-induced reinstatement after extinction. The authors found that inactivation of BLA, but not CeA, increased context-induced relapse in context A after punishment in context B and that inactivation of either BLA or CeA induced relapse in context B after punishment. In contrast, inactivation of either BLA or CeA decreased context-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking in context A after extinction in context B; the BLA results replicate results from a previous study (Fuchs et al., 2005). A surprising conclusion from this study is that, depending on the historical conditions that induce abstinence, amygdala activity can either promote or inhibit relapse.

Farrell et al. (2019) investigated the role of VP in relapse after punishment using Fos immunohistochemistry and DREADD inhibition in male and female rats. They first trained rats to self-administer cocaine in context A with a discrete cue paired with cocaine infusions and then exposed the rats to punishment-induced abstinence in context B in the presence of the discrete cue. Next, they tested the rats for context-induced relapse in context A in the presence or absence of the discrete cue and tested for relapse induced by cocaine priming in context A. Subsequently, they exposed different groups of rats to context A with or without the discrete cue, to context B without the cue, or did not expose to either context or cue (home cage) and determined Fos expression in different subregions of VP. The authors also divided the rats into punishment-sensitive and punishment-resistant groups based on the maximal shock intensity that induced abstinence in context B. They found that context-induced relapse was stronger when lever presses were reinforced with the discrete cues and that individual differences in punishment responding are associated with increased context-induced relapse but not cocaine priming–induced relapse. DREADD inhibition of VP decreased both context- and drug-priming–induced relapse after punishment. Finally, in agreement with Pelloux et al. (2018a), VP Fos expression was similar after exposure context A versus context B. Together, the results of this study indicate a role of VP in both context- and cocaine priming–induced relapse of cocaine seeking after punishment.

c. Relapse to drug seeking after punishment and home-cage forced abstinence

Several studies investigated mechanisms of relapse to drug seeking after punishment-imposed abstinence and subsequent home-cage forced abstinence. Early studies reported that, like extinction (Bouton and Swartzentruber, 1991), the punishment-suppressed conditioned response spontaneously recovers with passage of time (Azrin and Holz, 1966). Thus, this variation of the punishment-induced abstinence model mimics features of the human condition of resumption of drug taking over time after termination of the punishment contingencies that have maintained abstinence (e.g., termination of mandatory urine samples in which drug detection results in loss of employment or visitation rights of one’s children).

i. Alcohol Studies

Campbell et al. (2019a) used alcohol-preferring male rats and the ABA punishment-based procedure (Marchant et al., 2013a) to investigate the time course of relapse at different days after punishment cessation. They found that after 1 day of home-cage abstinence, relapse to alcohol seeking is context-specific and only occurs in context A, replicating previous findings (Marchant et al., 2013a, 2014, 2016). In contrast, after 30 days of home-cage abstinence, relapse reliably occurred in both context A and context B (punishment context), reflecting spontaneous recovery or incubation of the conditioned response in the punishment context (Krasnova et al., 2014). In follow-up experiments, they found that relapse in context B after 30 days of home-cage abstinence is associated with increased Fos expression in AI but not in other brain regions. Additionally, on abstinence day 30, muscimol + baclofen inactivation of AI decreased incubated relapse in context B. In contrast, on abstinence day 1, AI inactivation had no effect on context-induced relapse in context A or nonincubated low responding in context B. A question for future research is whether AI activity also plays a role in relapse in context A after prolonged home-cage abstinence.

Finally, in another study using a similar experimental design, Campbell et al. (2019b) first trained alcohol-preferring male rats to self-administer alcohol in context A. Next, the rats underwent punishment in context B and tested for relapse in context A or B. The authors then exposed groups of rats to either standard paired housing or enriched environment (large cages with enrichment items) for 31 days and retested them for relapse in both contexts on day 32. They reported that enrichment decreased relapse in both contexts. However, unlike the authors’ previous findings (Campbell et al., 2019a), incubation of lever presses in context B did not occur in the standard paired-housing group because of the large individual differences in context B responding both 1 and 32 days after punishment. The reasons for the large individual differences in context B responding are unknown.

ii. Cocaine Studies

Economidou et al. (2009) investigated whether individual differences in impulsivity (assessed in the five-choice serial reaction-time task) and cocaine exposure history (extended versus limited access) predict relapse 7 days after punishment-induced abstinence. The authors screened male rats for high and low impulsivity on the five-choice serial reaction-time task, and the impulsive phenotype was determined with respect to their total number of premature responses (responses made before the onset of the target stimulus) during challenge sessions in which the intertrial interval period was increased from 5 seconds to 7 seconds. Next, the authors trained the rats to self-administer cocaine under a second-order reinforcement schedule. During punishment, foot shock was probabilistically paired with the seeking response on 50% of the trials, and on these trials, cocaine was not delivered. They found that relapse was higher in the high-impulsivity rats and that a history of extended-access cocaine self-administration selectively increased responding in these rats. They also reported that acute pretreatment with the norepinephrine uptake release inhibitor atomoxetine decreases cocaine relapse. Atomoxetine is a medication for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder that decreases impulsivity in animal models and humans (de Wit, 2009).

In a follow-up study using the same experimental procedure, Pelloux et al. (2013) investigated in male rats the effect of permanent pretraining excitotoxic lesions of anterior cingulate, prelimbic, infralimbic, OFC, or AI cortices on relapse 7 days after punishment-induced abstinence. They found that prelimbic lesions decrease relapse, whereas AI lesions increase relapse; lesions of the other areas had no effect. Additionally, the cortical lesions had no effect on cocaine self-administration or punishment-induced abstinence. In contrast, BLA lesions had no effect on cocaine self-administration but blocked punishment-induced abstinence. These rats were not tested for relapse. However, a limitation of the study is the use of pretraining permanent lesions. Thus, a question for future research is whether similar results would emerge after reversible inactivation of the different regions immediately prior to relapse tests. Based on a recent study showing that acute reversible inactivation of AI decreases relapse to alcohol seeking after punishment plus prolonged home-cage abstinence in alcohol-preferring male rats (Campbell et al., 2018), we suspect that the results will be different.

iii. Methamphetamine Studies

Krasnova et al. (2014) investigated incubation of methamphetamine and palatable food seeking after extended-access self-administration (9 hours per day for 14 sessions) and punishment-induced suppression of drug or food self-administration (9 days) in male rats. Punishment-induced suppression of self-administration was achieved in all rats by increasing the daily shock level from 0.12 mA to 0.6 mA. Next, the authors tested the rats for relapse to drug or food seeking after 2 and 21 days of home-cage forced abstinence. The authors found time-dependent increases in both methamphetamine and food seeking (incubation of craving after punishment). As in previous studies (Li et al., 2015b, 2018; Grimm, 2020), they also found incubation of methamphetamine and food seeking after home-cage forced abstinence.

Torres et al. (2017) modified the incubation of drug craving after punishment procedure to investigate whether individual differences in punishment suppression correlate with incubation of methamphetamine seeking in male rats. In the modified procedure, shock level was progressively increased from 0.18 to 0.42 mA, resulting in two groups of rats: punishment-resistant (∼45% suppression) and punishment-sensitive (∼90% suppression). The authors found that incubation of methamphetamine seeking was higher in the punishment-resistant rats.

Krasnova et al. (2017) used a variation of the punishment procedure described above (shock levels were increased from 0.18 to 0.3 mA) to investigate gene expression changes in dorsal and ventral striatum after incubation of drug seeking in punishment-resistant (∼15%–20% suppression) and punishment-sensitive (∼80%–90% suppression) male rats. They used Affymetrix array platform containing 68,842 probes and measured different gene transcripts 24 hours after the day 30 relapse test. They replicated the findings that incubation of methamphetamine seeking after punishment is stronger in punishment-resistant rats. They also reported many gene expression differences between the punishment-resistant and punishment-sensitive rats in dorsal (e.g., CARTpt, the gene encoding cocaine- and amphetamine-regulated transcript protein) and ventral (e.g., oxytocin) striatum. However, the relevance of these correlational findings to mechanisms of incubation of methamphetamine craving after punishment is unknown because the authors have not followed up on the correlational results with causal role site-specific pharmacological or viral gene knockdown manipulations.

Hu et al. (2019) used the same punishment-resistance/sensitive experimental procedure used by Krasnova et al. (2017) and included a rat fMRI procedure to longitudinally investigate circuit connectivity changes during methamphetamine self-administration, punishment responding in punishment-resistant and punishment-sensitive male rats, and incubation of drug seeking after punishment. They reported that over time fMRI signaling increases in OFC-medial striatum projection and decreases in dorsal mPFC-NAc projection in punishment-resistant but not punishment-sensitive rats. These are interesting correlational data, but most likely they are not relevant to understanding of incubation because, unlike the studies described above, no differences in incubation of methamphetamine seeking were observed between punishment-resistant and punishment-sensitive rats. Additionally, even within the context of circuit changes that control resistance to punishment (Vanderschuren et al., 2017), the results should be interpreted with caution in the absence of projection-specific inhibition/activation experiments to determine the projections’ role in punishment responding. This is because an inherent confound in the authors’ experimental procedure is that punishment-resistant rats are exposed to both more methamphetamine and foot shock than the punishment-sensitive rats. Thus, the observed circuit changes may be due to group differences in both drug and stress exposure, both of which cause long-lasting brain changes (Kalivas and Stewart, 1991; Nestler and Aghajanian, 1997; McEwen et al., 2016).

3. Conclusions

Investigators have developed experimental procedures to investigate mechanisms of relapse after punishment-induced abstinence. The different procedural variations of the punishment-induced abstinence model can be used to study different forms of relapse of drug seeking. In one procedural variation, punishment can be used as a substitute for extinction to investigate the effect of manipulations traditionally used in reinstatement studies (discrete cue, context, and drug priming) on relapse after punishment-induced abstinence (Panlilio et al., 2005; Marchant et al., 2019). In another procedural variation, investigators can study relapse after punishment-induced abstinence and different periods of home-cage abstinence (incubation of drug craving after punishment) (Krasnova et al., 2014).

A main question for future research is whether mechanisms of relapse are similar after punishment-induced abstinence versus extinction-induced abstinence. The data indicate both similarities and differences. Heroin priming injection induces relapse after either punishment or extinction, whereas lorazepam’s effect is selective to punishment (Panlilio et al., 2005). LH, vSub, NAc shell, vSub to NAc shell projections are critical for context-induced relapse after either punishment (Fig. 2) or extinction (Marchant et al., 2019). In contrast, reversible inactivation of BLA has opposite effects on context-induced relapse after punishment (potentiation) versus extinction (inhibition), whereas CeA inactivation decreases relapse after extinction but not punishment (Pelloux et al., 2018b). Another question for future studies is whether the findings discussed above generalize to female rats. To date, with the exception of Farrell et al. (2019), who used both males and females, all studies using the punishment-induced abstinence model only included male rats.

Fig. 2
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 2

Brain regions and projections involved in relapse after voluntary abstinence induced by adverse consequences (A-B) or food and social choice (C-D) for different addictive drugs. AIC, AI cortex; BLA, basolateral amygdala; CeA, central nucleus of amygdala; dmPFC, dorsomedial prefrontal cortex; DMS, dorsomedial striatum; LH, lateral hypothalamus; NAc, nucleus accumbens; OFC, orbitofrontal cortex; Pir, piriform cortex; vSub, ventral subiculum.

B. Electric Barrier–Induced Voluntary Abstinence Model

1. Overview

The procedure includes three phases: drug self-administration, electric barrier–induced voluntary abstinence, and relapse tests. During the training phase, laboratory animals are trained to self-administer a drug; each drug delivery is paired with a discrete cue. Next, during the voluntary abstinence phase, drug-taking behavior is suppressed by introducing an electric barrier of increasing intensity in front of the drug-paired lever; the rats must cross this barrier to gain access to the drug (Cooper et al., 2007). During testing, typically performed 1 day after the completion of voluntary abstinence, relapse is assessed in the presence or absence of the electric barrier (Cooper et al., 2007). As with punishment-induced abstinence, relapse to drug seeking after electric barrier–induced abstinence can be tested after different abstinence periods to investigate incubation of drug craving (Fredriksson et al., 2020).

In the electric barrier–based relapse model, the negative consequences are associated with drug seeking (the electric barrier is adjacent to the drug-paired lever) prior to drug taking. The model attempts to mimic abstinence in humans due to negative consequences of drug seeking (e.g., securing money to obtain drug, stressful interactions with a drug dealer, adverse interactions with law enforcement) (Cooper et al., 2007). The electric barrier–based relapse model [also termed a conflict model (Cooper et al., 2007)] is based on the Columbia obstruction box method that was used many years ago to assess rats’ motivation to obtain rewards under different deprivation conditions in the presence of an electric barrier (Jenkins et al., 1926; Warden, 1931). Relapse after electric barrier–induced abstinence has been studied in rats trained to self-administer cocaine, heroin, and oxycodone and has only been used to investigate discrete cue–induced relapse and incubation of drug craving (Table 4).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
TABLE 4

Relapse after electric barrier–induced voluntary abstinence: summary of main findings

2. Review of Studies

a. Discrete cue–induced relapse in the presence of the electric barrier

In an initial study, Cooper et al. (2007) trained male rats to lever press for cocaine infusions that were paired with a discrete light cue. Next, they introduced an electric barrier and increased the barrier intensity over days from 0.25 mA up to 0.45 mA until the rats voluntarily abstained from cocaine self-administration for 3 days. During the subsequent relapse tests, they exposed the rats to intermittent noncontingent light-cue presentations (every 5 minutes for 20 seconds) and measured resumption of lever responding in the presence of the electric barrier intensity that led to 3 voluntary abstinence days. During testing, lever presses led to contingent light-cue presentations but not cocaine. The authors found that noncontingent cue exposure led to resumption of lever presses during the relapse tests, with large individual differences in responding in the rats who crossed the barrier during testing. The large individual differences during the relapse test agrees with other studies in which electric shock was used to suppress cocaine-taking behavior (Deroche-Gamonet et al., 2004; Pelloux et al., 2007). In these studies, only a relatively small proportion of the rats (less than 25%) continued to engage in drug-taking behavior in the presence of the aversive stimulus.

In a follow-up study, Barnea-Ygael et al. (2012) used a similar experimental procedure, except that they tested the male rats for relapse in the presence of either maximal-intensity electrical barrier or 85% of this intensity, measured relapse either 1 or 14 days after the last electric barrier exposure, and compared cocaine relapse to sucrose relapse. They reported that relapse responding was higher when electric barrier is at 85% intensity, responding for the cocaine cue was higher than responding for the sucrose cue, and responding was lower after 14 days of home-cage forced abstinence. The latter finding was unexpected based on the findings from many studies on incubation of cocaine craving after home-cage forced abstinence (Grimm et al., 2001; Wolf, 2016; Dong et al., 2017). A potential reason for this discrepancy is the development of time-dependent sensitization to electric barrier exposure (a stress condition) and cues associated with the electric barrier (Antelman et al., 2000) under the authors’ experimental conditions. This can result in the development of incubation of conditioned fear (Pickens et al., 2009), which competes with the development of incubation of cocaine craving. Indeed, when the authors performed a second relapse test in the absence of the electric barrier during early (day 2) and late abstinence (day 15), responding was somewhat higher during late abstinence.

Peck et al. (2013) used the procedure developed by Cooper et al. (2007) to compare cue-induced relapse after electric barrier–induced voluntary abstinence in male rats trained to self-administer cocaine or heroin. One procedural difference was that during the relapse tests, the rats were only exposed to noncontingent cue presentations every 5 minutes, but lever presses were not reinforced by the cue. They reported that although cue-induced relapse was observed in all heroin-trained rats (10 of 10), responding was much more variable in the cocaine-trained rats (only 3 of 8), confirming the observations of large individual differences observed in the studies of Cooper et al. (2007) and Barnea-Ygael et al. (2012).

Together, heroin-trained male rats appear more vulnerable to cue-induced relapse after electric barrier–induced abstinence. A possible explanation for greater relapse in heroin- versus cocaine-trained rats might be the different motivational effects of heroin- and cocaine-associated cues during both self-administration and electric barrier suppression (see section IV.B for discussion).

Saunders et al. (2013) measured cue-induced relapse to cocaine seeking in male rats, identified as sign-trackers and goal-trackers based on their behavioral response in a Pavlovian conditioned approach procedure in which a lever extension (the conditioned stimulus) predicts the delivery of food (unconditioned stimulus) to a nearby receptacle (Peterson et al., 1972). Sign-trackers are rats that respond to the lever conditioned stimulus by interacting with it (e.g., licking, biting); goal-trackers are rats that respond to the lever conditioned stimulus extension by approaching the location where the food unconditioned stimulus would be delivered (i.e., the food receptacle) (Peterson et al., 1972). The authors reported that the sign-trackers showed higher cue-induced relapse to cocaine seeking after electric barrier–induced abstinence, a finding that agrees with the authors’ previous finding of higher discrete cue–induced reinstatement after extinction in these rats (Saunders and Robinson, 2010). The authors also reported that NAc core injections of the nonselective dopamine receptor antagonist flupentixol decrease cue-induced relapse to cocaine seeking after electric barrier–induced abstinence, whereas local amphetamine injections increase relapse. In both cases, the effects were more pronounced in sign-trackers than in goal-trackers. These results demonstrate a critical role of NAc core dopamine in cue-induced relapse to cocaine seeking after electric barrier–induced abstinence (Saunders et al., 2013).

Finally, in a more recent study, Ewing et al. (2021) used a variation of the electric barrier model to study the effect of systemic injections of a Drd3 receptor antagonist (NGB 2904) and a Drd1 receptor partial agonist (SKF 77434) on cue-induced relapse to heroin seeking in the presence of the electrical barrier (set to 25% of the intensity for each rat during abstinence) after electric barrier suppression of heroin self-administration in male rats. They reported that a combination of low doses of each drug, which were independently ineffective, decreased cue-induced heroin seeking after electric barrier–induced abstinence.

b. Incubation of opioid craving

Fredriksson et al. (2020) modified the electric barrier-relapse model to study incubation of opioid (oxycodone) craving after voluntary abstinence due to negative consequences of drug seeking. An important difference between this procedural variation of the electric barrier model is that testing is performed in the absence of the electric barrier and active lever presses during testing lead to contingent presentation of a compound discrete cue (tone and light). In an initial study, Fredriksson et al. (2020) compared incubation of oxycodone seeking after electric barrier–induced abstinence with incubation after home-cage forced abstinence. The authors trained male and female rats to self-administer oxycodone (6 hours per day) for 14 days. They then exposed them to either home-cage forced abstinence or voluntary abstinence induced by an electric barrier of increasing intensity (from 0.1 mA to 0.4 mA) near the drug-paired lever. On abstinence days 1, 15, or 30, the authors tested the rats for oxycodone seeking without shock and drug. They found that, independent of sex, the time-dependent increase in oxycodone seeking after cessation of opioid self-administration (incubation of opioid craving) was stronger after electric barrier–induced abstinence than after forced abstinence (Fredriksson et al., 2020) (Fig. 3). These results suggest that abstinence due to negative consequences would increase relapse vulnerability. The reasons for this potentiated effect are unknown, but the authors speculated that a potential reason might be due to stress exposure during the electric barrier phase. This speculation agrees with a previous study showing that repeated restraint stress exposure during forced abstinence increases incubation of cocaine seeking (Glynn et al., 2018).

Fig. 3
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 3

Schematic representation of the effect of different abstinence models on incubation of drug craving. Drug-seeking intensity during relapse tests after prolonged abstinence is represented as an increasing spectrum ranging from white (low drug seeking) to red (high drug seeking). The figure depicts how positive alternative rewards during abstinence (e.g., social interaction or palatable food pellets) tend to be protective (left portion). Conversely, no alternatives (e.g., forced abstinence) or negative alternatives (e.g., electric barrier–induced voluntary abstinence) tend to promote drug seeking (right portion).

Fredriksson et al. (2020) also determined whether the dopamine stabilizer (−)-OSU6162 [a potential addiction treatment medication (Khemiri et al., 2015)] would decrease incubation of oxycodone seeking after forced or voluntary abstinence in male and female rats. (−)-OSU6162 was developed by Carlsson and colleagues, and the drug can stimulate or inhibit dopamine-related behaviors depending on dopaminergic tone (Sonesson et al., 1994; Natesan et al., 2006; Rung et al., 2008). The authors found that (−)-OSU6162 decreases incubation of oxycodone seeking after voluntary abstinence, an effect that was stronger in male rats. (−)-OSU6162 also decreased incubation of oxycodone seeking after forced abstinence in males but not females. (−)-OSU6162’s effect on oxycodone seeking was only observed on abstinence day 15 but not day 1, suggesting a selective effect on incubated opioid seeking that, at least in males, is independent of the method used to achieve abstinence (Fredriksson et al., 2020). The reasons for the lack of effect in female rats are unknown. The authors suggested that the sex differences in (−)-OSU6162’s effect may be due to sex differences in dopamine function and responses to dopaminergic drugs (Robinson and Becker, 1986; Roth et al., 2004; Becker and Hu, 2008) or potential sex differences in (−)-OSU6162 pharmacokinetics.

In another study, Fredriksson et al. (2021) determined the role of vSub in incubation of oxycodone seeking after electric barrier–induced abstinence in male and female rats. They tested the rats for relapse to oxycodone seeking on abstinence day 15 and extracted their brains for Fos immunohistochemistry or tested the rats after vSub injections of vehicle or muscimol + baclofen on abstinence day 1 or day 15 after forced or electric barrier–induced abstinence. The authors found that relapse after electric barrier–induced abstinence was associated with increased Fos expression in vSub and that local inactivation of vSub decreases incubated oxycodone seeking on day 15. In contrast, local inactivation of vSub on abstinence day 1 or day 15 had no effect on nonincubated oxycodone seeking or incubated oxycodone seeking after forced abstinence. In the same study, the authors used theDaun02 chemogenetic inactivation procedure (Koya et al., 2009; Bossert et al., 2011) in Fos-LacZ transgenic rats to selectively inactivate the relapse (incubation) test-activated Fos-expressing neurons in vSub. They found that Daun02 inactivation decreased incubated oxycodone seeking, indicating a role of vSub neural ensembles in incubation after electric barrier–induced abstinence.

3. Conclusions

We and others developed experimental procedures to investigate mechanisms of relapse after electric barrier–induced abstinence. As with the punishment-based procedures, electric barrier can be used to investigate the effect on drug seeking of manipulations traditionally used in reinstatement studies (discrete cue, context, and drug priming) on relapse (Cooper et al., 2007) or to investigate incubation of drug craving (Fredriksson et al., 2020) after abstinence due to negative consequences of drug seeking.

In the electric barrier model, relapse can be assessed either in the presence or the absence of the electric barrier, and this methodological parameter can have a significant effect on relapse behavior. For example, large individual differences in relapse vulnerability are observed in the presence of the electric barrier (Cooper et al., 2007; Barnea-Ygael et al., 2012) but not in the absence of the barrier (Fredriksson et al., 2020). These individual differences can be explained in part by pre-existing individual differences in responding to food reward conditioned stimuli in classic Pavlovian approach procedures (Saunders et al., 2013). Another potential reason for the individual differences observed in the presence of the electric barrier (Cooper et al., 2007; Barnea-Ygael et al., 2012) is the use of a single noncontingent cue versus a contingent compound cue (tone + light) in the Fredriksson et al. (2020) study. Previous studies have shown that noncontingent cues do not reliably reinstate cocaine seeking after extinction and that a compound discrete cue is more effective than a single cue (See et al., 1999; Kruzich et al., 2001).

Additionally, time-dependent decreases in relapse to drug seeking during abstinence are observed in the presence of the barrier (Barnea-Ygael et al., 2012), whereas the opposite occurs in the absence of the barrier (Fredriksson et al., 2020). However, a potential alternative explanation for the lack of incubation in the Barnea-Ygael et al. (2012) study is the use of a short-access training procedure (3 hours per day) versus a long-access training procedure (6 hours per day) in the Fredriksson et al. (2020) study. Indeed, Lu et al. (2004) reported that incubation of cue-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking after extinction is more robust after extended-access (6 hours per day) versus limited-access (2 hours per day) cocaine self-administration.

Finally, results from neuropharmacological studies indicate that dopamine is critical to both cue-induced relapse to cocaine and heroin seeking and incubation of oxycodone seeking after electric barrier–induced voluntary abstinence (Saunders et al., 2013; Fredriksson et al., 2020; Ewing et al., 2021). In the case of cue-induced relapse to cocaine seeking, a critical brain region is the NAc core (Saunders et al., 2013). Additionally, recent data indicate a role of vSub in potentiated incubation of opioid seeking after electric barrier–induced abstinence (Fig. 2) (Fredriksson et al., 2020). However, to date, very few neuropharmacological studies assessed relapse after electric barrier–induced abstinence, and only one peer-reviewed study included females. In this study, females were less sensitive than males to the antirelapse effects of the dopamine stabilizer, (−)-OSU6162 (Fredriksson et al., 2020), which might be due to sex differences in dopamine function and responses to dopaminergic drugs (Robinson and Becker, 1986; Roth et al., 2004; Becker and Hu, 2008).

III. Relapse after Abstinence Induced by Availability of Alternative Nondrug Rewards

A. Food Choice–Induced Voluntary Abstinence Model

1. Overview

The procedure includes four phases: food and drug self-administration, early abstinence relapse test, food choice–induced voluntary abstinence, and late abstinence relapse test (Caprioli et al., 2015a). During the training phase, laboratory animals are first trained to self-administer palatable food pellets and then to self-administer a drug; each reward is paired with unique discriminative and discrete cues. During the early abstinence relapse test, the subjects are tested for drug seeking (lever presses are reinforced by the drug-associated discrete cues but not the drug, extinction conditions). During the voluntary abstinence phase, drug self-administration is suppressed by providing the subjects multiple daily mutually exclusive choices between the drug and the palatable food (Fig. 1). During the late abstinence relapse test, performed 1 day after the completion of the voluntary abstinence phase, the subjects are tested again for relapse to drug seeking. During the relapse tests, the food reward alternative is not available.

The procedure can be used to investigate incubation of drug craving after food choice–induced voluntary abstinence (Caprioli et al., 2015a; Venniro et al., 2017b) or relapse after voluntary abstinence when drug seeking is not assessed during early abstinence (Venniro et al., 2017a; Reiner et al., 2020) (Table 5). The development of the food choice voluntary abstinence/relapse model was based on previous food choice studies using nonhuman primates (Nader and Banks, 2014; Banks and Negus, 2017). These choice studies were primarily used to study pharmacological mechanisms of drug reinforcement, but Gasior et al. (2004) suggested that these models can also be used to study relapse to drug use. The food choice voluntary abstinence model (Caprioli et al., 2015b) was also inspired by studies of the Ahmed group showing that rats strongly prefer saccharin or sucrose over cocaine or heroin (Lenoir et al., 2007; Lenoir et al., 2013; Ahmed, 2018). Caprioli et al. (2015b) independently replicated this effect and reported that male rats show strong preference for palatable high-carbohydrate food pellets over methamphetamine under multiple experimental conditions.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
TABLE 5

Relapse after food choice–induced voluntary abstinence: summary of main findings

From a translational perspective, the food choice–induced abstinence model attempts to mimic some aspects of abstinence in humans that is achieved due to availability of nondrug rewards that are chosen over the addictive drug (Caprioli et al., 2015a). This principle is exemplified by contingency management, a behavioral treatment in which small prizes or monetary vouchers can maintain abstinence for many months; however, when contingency management is discontinued, humans often relapse to drug use (Preston et al., 2002; Higgins et al., 2004).

2. Review of Studies

a. Methamphetamine studies

In the first study, Caprioli et al. (2015a) trained male rats to self-administer palatable food (six sessions, 9 or 3 hours per day) and then to self-administer methamphetamine under two different self-administration procedures that are widely used to model drug addiction: extended-daily-access drug self-administration procedure (12 sessions, 9 hours per day) (Ahmed and Koob, 1998) and the three-criteria DSM-IV model (Deroche-Gamonet et al., 2004), used to identify “addicted” rats after long-term training (50 sessions, 3 hours per day). The authors then assessed methamphetamine seeking in relapse tests after 1 or 21 abstinence days. Between tests, the rats underwent either home-cage forced abstinence or voluntary abstinence for 19 days (achieved via a discrete-choice procedure between methamphetamine and palatable food; 20 trials per day). The authors found that under both training protocols (short-term extended daily access or long-term limited daily access) and abstinence conditions (forced and voluntary), methamphetamine seeking in the relapse tests was higher after 21 abstinence days than after 1 day (incubation of methamphetamine craving). These results indicate that incubation after food choice–induced voluntary abstinence is as robust as incubation after forced abstinence. In the same study, Caprioli et al. (2015a) also determined the effect of the novel mGluR2 (metabotropic glutamate receptor 2) positive allosteric modulator, AZD8529 (Justinova et al., 2015), on relapse to methamphetamine seeking after forced or voluntary abstinence in male rats. AZD8529 decreased methamphetamine seeking on day 21 but not day 1 of forced or voluntary abstinence, indicating a specific effect of AZD8529 on incubated methamphetamine seeking that is independent of abstinence condition (voluntary or forced).

In a follow-up study, Venniro et al. (2017b) used a similar experimental procedure (except that the duration of the training sessions was 6 hours per day) to determine the generality of the voluntary abstinence choice model to female rats and to determine whether there are sex differences in incubation of drug seeking after food choice–induced abstinence. The authors found that incubation of methamphetamine seeking generalizes to female rats and that there are no sex differences in incubation of methamphetamine seeking. However, the authors observed a small decrease in food choice in female rats trained to self-administered methamphetamine (but not heroin) compared with male rats.

In the first study on brain mechanisms of incubation of methamphetamine craving after food choice–induced abstinence, Caprioli et al. (2017) explored the role of DLS and DMS in this incubation in male rats. The authors chose these striatal subregions because Li et al. (2015b) previously found that their activity is critical to incubation of methamphetamine seeking after home-cage forced abstinence. Using RNAscope, the authors found that incubation of methamphetamine seeking after food choice–induced abstinence is associated with increased Fos in DMS but not DLS and that Fos was coexpressed on both Drd1 and dopamine D2 receptor (Drd2)-expressing cells. Next, the authors found that DMS injections of a Drd1 antagonist (SCH 39166) or a Drd2 antagonist (raclopride) selectively decrease incubated relapse on abstinence day 21 but not nonincubated relapse on day 1. Finally, in the same study, Caprioli et al. (2017) used the Daun02 chemogenetic inactivation procedure (Koya et al., 2009; Bossert et al., 2011) in Fos-LacZ transgenic rats to selectively inactivate the relapse test-activated Fos-expressing neurons in DMS to demonstrate a causal role of these neurons (the putative neuronal ensembles) in incubation of methamphetamine seeking. Together, the results of this study demonstrate a role of DMS Drd1- and Drd2-expressing neurons in incubation of methamphetamine seeking after voluntary abstinence and that DMS neuronal ensembles, which comprise both neuronal populations, are critical for this incubation.

In a follow-up study, Rossi et al. (2020) investigated the role of NAc core and shell Drd1 and Drd2 in incubation of methamphetamine craving after food choice–induced abstinence in male rats. They used experimental procedures similar to those used in the studies described above except that the alternative nondrug reward was a palatable solution (sucrose 1% + maltodextrin 1%) and incubation was assessed after 15 abstinence days. Using RNAscope, they reported that incubation of methamphetamine seeking is associated with increased Fos in Drd1- and Drd2-expressing cells in NAc core but not shell. Additionally, NAc core but not shell injections of muscimol + baclofen, flupentixol, SCH 39166, and raclopride decreased incubated methamphetamine seeking after 15 abstinence days. Together, these results indicate that dopamine transmission through Drd1 and Drd2 in NAc core is critical to incubation of methamphetamine seeking after voluntary abstinence.

Finally, Venniro et al. (2017a) used the food choice–induced voluntary abstinence model to investigate relapse after limited-access (2-hour sessions per day) methamphetamine self-administration in male rats. Under these conditions, drug seeking does not incubate over time (unpublished data). The goal of the study was to determine the role of CeA and glutamatergic projections to this region in relapse after 15 days of food choice–induced abstinence. The authors found that relapse after voluntary abstinence was associated with higher Fos expression in CeA Drd1-expressing neurons than in Drd2-expressing neurons, with a similar pattern of activation in lateral (CeL) and medial (CeM) parts. Additionally, systemic injections of a Drd1 antagonist (SCH 39166) decreased both relapse and relapse-associated increases in CeA activity. The systemic effect of SCH 39166 was mimicked by CeA drug injections; in contrast, CeA injections of a Drd2 antagonist (raclopride) or BLA injections of SCH 39166 were ineffective. Furthermore, relapse after voluntary abstinence was associated with selective activation of the ventral AI (AIV) projection to CeA but not projections from vSub, PVT, vmPFC, and BLA. Reversible inactivation of the AIV (but not the nearby OFC) and chemogenetic inhibition of the AIV→CeA projection decreased relapse, and inhibition of the AIV→CeA projection also decreased CeA Fos expression. Finally, in the same study, Venniro et al. (2017a) used electron microscopy and electrophysiology to demonstrate that AIV vGluT1 (vesicular glutamate transporter 1)-expressing projection neurons preferentially innervate the CeL subregion of CeA and form monosynaptic glutamatergic asymmetric synapses on CeA cells. Together, these results demonstrate a critical role of Drd1-mediated neuronal activity in CeA, which is controlled by AIV→CeA glutamatergic projections, in relapse to methamphetamine seeking after food choice–induced abstinence.

b. Opioid studies

Venniro et al. (2017b) compared incubation of heroin seeking in male and female rats after food choice–induced abstinence versus home-cage forced abstinence. The experimental procedure was identical to the one used in the methamphetamine studies described above except that lever presses were reinforced with heroin during the 6-hour drug self-administration sessions. As in previous studies (Shalev et al., 2001; Airavaara et al., 2011), the authors found reliable incubation of heroin seeking after forced abstinence (higher drug seeking on abstinence day 21). In contrast, food choice–induced voluntary abstinence prevented the emergence of incubation of heroin seeking in both male and female rats. Finally, under the authors’ experimental conditions, there were no sex differences in the strong preference for the palatable food over heroin, incubation of heroin seeking after forced abstinence, or the prevention of incubation of heroin seeking after food-induced abstinence (Venniro et al., 2017b).

In a recent study, Reiner et al. (2020) showed that the inhibitory effect of food choice–induced voluntary abstinence on incubation of heroin seeking generalizes to the synthetic opioid fentanyl. In this study, the authors investigated the role of OFC and its afferent projections in relapse to fentanyl seeking after 2 weeks of food choice–induced abstinence. The authors studied the OFC because, in a previous study, Fanous et al. (2012) demonstrated a role of this brain region in incubation of heroin seeking after forced abstinence. Reiner et al. (2020) found that in both male and female rats, relapse after food choice–induced abstinence is associated with increased Fos expression in OFC and that reversible inactivation of OFC with muscimol + baclofen decreases relapse. The authors then determined projection-specific activation of OFC afferents during the relapse test by using Fos plus CTb (injected into OFC). They found that relapse to fentanyl seeking is associated with increased Fos expression in piriform cortex (Pir) neurons projecting to OFC but not in projections from BLA and thalamus. Next, the authors found that inactivation of Pir with muscimol + baclofen decreases relapse. This effect was mimicked by anatomic disconnection of Pir from OFC by unilateral muscimol + baclofen injections into Pir in one hemisphere plus unilateral muscimol + baclofen injections into OFC in the contralateral side. Finally, relapse to fentanyl seeking after food choice–induced abstinence was associated with increased Fos in AI, and reversible inactivation of the AI decreased relapse. Together, the results identify a role of OFC, Pir, AI, and Pir-OFC projections in relapse to fentanyl seeking after food-induced abstinence.

3. Conclusions

Caprioli et al. (2015a) established a reliable procedure to study mechanisms of relapse to psychostimulant and opioid seeking after food choice–induced voluntary abstinence in male and female rats. At the behavioral level, no sex differences were observed in the strong preference for the palatable food over the drugs or in relapse after food choice–induced abstinence except for a small decrease in food choice in female rats trained to self-administered methamphetamine (but not heroin) compared with male rats.

An unexpected finding from the studies discussed above is that food choice–induced abstinence has no effect on incubation of methamphetamine seeking but prevents the emergence of incubation of opioid (heroin and fentanyl) seeking. The reasons for the selective effect of food choice on heroin versus methamphetamine relapse are unknown and a question for future research.

Finally, mechanistic studies demonstrate a role of DMS (but not DLS) and NAc core (but not shell) in incubation of methamphetamine seeking after food choice–induced abstinence. We speculate that this common role suggests that relapse after choice-induced abstinence reflects goal-directed (devaluation sensitive) behavior for which activity of both DMS and NAc core (but not DLS) is critical (Balleine and O’Doherty, 2010; Parkes et al., 2015). There is also evidence for a role of CeA Drd1, AIV (but not OFC) activity, and the projection from AIV to CeA in relapse to methamphetamine seeking after food choice–induced abstinence (Fig. 2). Finally, the recent study by Reiner et al. (2020) indicates a role of OFC, AI, Pir, and projections between Pir and OFC in relapse to fentanyl seeking after food choice–induced abstinence (Fig. 2). The selective role of OFC in relapse to fentanyl but not methamphetamine seeking suggests that the brain circuits that control relapse to psychostimulant versus opioid drugs after food choice–induced abstinence are not the same.

B. Social Choice–Induced Voluntary Abstinence Model

1. Overview

The procedure includes four phases: social interaction and drug self-administration, early abstinence relapse test, social choice–induced voluntary abstinence, and late abstinence relapse test (Venniro et al., 2018; Venniro and Shaham, 2020). During the training phase, laboratory animals are first trained to lever press for access to social interaction with a peer and then to self-administer a drug; each reward is paired with unique discriminative and discrete cues. During the early abstinence relapse test, the subjects are tested for drug seeking (lever presses are reinforced by the discrete drug-associated cues but not the drug). During the voluntary abstinence phase, drug self-administration is suppressed by providing the subjects daily mutually exclusive choices between the drug and social interaction (social reward). During the late abstinence relapse test, performed 1 day after the completion of the voluntary abstinence phase, the subjects are tested again for relapse to drug seeking. During the relapse tests, the social-reward alternative is not available (Venniro and Shaham, 2020).

Venniro et al. (2018) developed the social choice procedure because the use of palatable food as the nondrug reward may limit the clinical translation of choice models. In most human drug users, the rewards that compete with drugs are primarily social (family and employment) (Stitzer et al., 2011). From a translation perspective, the social choice model attempts to mimic some aspects of human behavioral treatments such as the community reinforcement approach and the therapeutic work place, which promote prolonged abstinence by offering volitional social interactions with social reinforcers such as support groups and positive work environments (Hunt and Azrin, 1973; Silverman et al., 2012). A major finding from the initial study introducing the social choice model was that rats strongly prefer social interaction over drugs and that this effect was independent of “addiction score,” drug (opioid versus psychostimulant), dose, self-administration procedure, housing conditions (single versus paired housing), sex, and duration of time in home cage (forced abstinence) (Venniro et al., 2018) (see Table 6 for a detailed description). Below, we describe results from the recent studies on incubation of drug craving after social choice–induced voluntary abstinence (Table 7).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
TABLE 6

The impact of social reward on drug self-administration in different animal models of addiction

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
TABLE 7

Relapse after social choice–induced voluntary abstinence: summary of main findings

2. Review of Studies

In the first study, Venniro et al. (2018) trained rats to lever press for social interaction (six sessions, 2 hours per day, 60 trials per session) or palatable food (6 hours per day). Next, the authors trained the rats to lever press for methamphetamine for 12 sessions (6 hours per day). The authors then compared different groups of male and female rats for incubation of methamphetamine seeking after social choice–induced abstinence versus incubation after food choice–induced abstinence or forced abstinence. As in previous studies (see above), the authors found reliable incubation of drug seeking after either food choice–induced voluntary abstinence or forced abstinence. In contrast, incubation of methamphetamine seeking was prevented by social choice–induced voluntary abstinence (Fig. 3); this inhibitory effect persisted for an additional 30 days of home-cage forced abstinence (Venniro et al., 2018).

In two follow-up correlational experiments Venniro et al. (2018) used double and triple immunohistochemistry and RNAscope in situ hybridization and found that the protective effect of social choice–induced abstinence on incubation of methamphetamine seeking is associated with activation (assessed by Fos) of inhibitory protein kinase-Cδ (PKCδ)-expressing neurons in CeL and decreased activity of output neurons in CeM. In contrast, the strong incubation of drug seeking after forced abstinence was associated with activation of CeL-expressing somatostatin (SOM) neurons and CeM output neurons. The protective effect of social choice–induced abstinence on incubation was also associated with decreased activity (Fos expression) of AIV and dorsal AI, but not anterior cingulate, dorsal and ventral mPFC, lateral and medial OFC, and BLA (Venniro et al., 2018).

In a recent study, Venniro et al. (2020b) determined the causal role of CeL PKCδ and SOM in inhibition of incubation of methamphetamine seeking after social choice–induced abstinence and expression of incubation of drug seeking after home-cage forced abstinence, respectively. For this purpose, the authors used male rats and developed short-hairpin RNAs against PKCδ or SOM. In initial slice in vivo electrophysiology experiments, the authors found that viral knockdown of the PKCδ enzyme or SOM peptide in CeL inhibits neuronal activity (decreased firing in response to depolarizing current injections) in PKCδ-expressing or SOM-expressing cells, respectively. Next, in the behavioral experiments, the authors found that viral knockdown of PKCδ enzyme in CeL decreases Fos in CeL PKCδ-expressing neurons, increases Fos in CeM output neurons, and reverses the inhibitory effect of social choice–induced abstinence on incubation of methamphetamine seeking. In contrast, viral knockdown of SOM CeL injections decreased Fos in CeL SOM-expressing neurons, decreased Fos in CeM output neurons, and decreased incubation after forced abstinence (Venniro et al., 2020b). Together, these results demonstrate a causal role of CeL PKCδ activity in the protective effect of social choice–induced abstinence against the development of incubation of methamphetamine seeking and CeL SOM activity in the expression of incubation of methamphetamine seeking after home-cage forced abstinence.

Finally, in another study, Venniro et al. (2019) determined the generality of the protective effect of social choice–induced abstinence to incubation of heroin seeking. In the first experiment, the authors trained male and female rats for social self-administration (6 days) and then for extended-access heroin self-administration (12 days). Next, the authors determined incubation of heroin seeking after social choice–induced abstinence or forced abstinence. The authors found that incubation of heroin seeking was observed under both experimental conditions, but it was lower after social choice–induced abstinence than after forced abstinence. In the second experiment, the authors replicated the findings of incubation of heroin seeking after social choice–induced abstinence and also showed that this incubation occurs under conditions of more limited social interaction via a screen that separates the drug self-administration chamber and the social peer chamber; the screen allows physical contact but prevents rats from crossing between the chambers. Venniro et al. (2019) developed the fully automatic screen model to eliminate procedural limitations of the original semiautomatic model: intense workload and repeated physical interaction between the experimenter and rats, which can introduce experimenter-related confounds and cause rodent-related allergies to the experimenter (Venniro and Shaham, 2020).

3. Conclusions

Venniro et al. (2018) established a reliable procedure to study relapse to psychostimulant and opioid seeking after social choice–induced voluntary abstinence. As with the palatable food choice–induced abstinence model, they observed no sex differences in the strong preference for social interaction over the drugs or in relapse after social choice–induced abstinence. At the behavioral level, there are two main findings. The first is the unexpected strong inhibitory effect of social choice–induced abstinence on incubation of methamphetamine seeking but not incubation of heroin seeking. This finding was unexpected because this pattern of results is opposite from what was observed after food choice–induced abstinence: prevention of incubation of heroin seeking and no effect on incubation of methamphetamine seeking. The reasons for this double dissociation between the drug type and the alternative reward are unknown. At this time, we feel it is too early to speculate about potential reasons. We hope that future mechanistic studies, particularly on mechanisms of incubation of drug craving after social versus food choice–induced abstinence across drug classes, will identify factors contributing to this double dissociation.

Finally, the mechanistic studies demonstrate a critical role of PKCδ-expressing neurons and the PKCδ enzyme in CeL and neuronal activity in yet-to-be-identified cell type(s) in CeM in the protective effect of social choice–induced abstinence on incubation of methamphetamine seeking (Fig. 3). A question for future research is what afferent and efferent CeL projections contribute to this protective effect. Based on the correlational Fos data showing that the prevention of incubation of social choice–induced abstinence is associated with decreased activity of AIV neurons, we speculate that decreased activity of the AIV→CeL glutamatergic projection (Venniro et al., 2017a) to drug-associated cues during the late abstinence relapse tests contributes to this protective effect.

IV. Conclusions and Clinical Implications

In this review, we describe different animal models that assess relapse to drug seeking after voluntary abstinence achieved either by introducing adverse consequences to the drug taking (punishment) or seeking (electric barrier) or by introducing operant nondrug rewards (palatable food or social interaction) in a mutually exclusive discrete-choice procedure. We also reviewed studies on behavioral and neuropharmacological mechanisms identified in studies using these voluntary abstinence relapse models. Below, we discuss similarities and differences in mechanisms of relapse after voluntary abstinence versus forced abstinence, similarities and differences in mechanisms of relapse after voluntary abstinence across drug classes, and the role of sex as a biological variable in relapse after voluntary abstinence, and we conclude by briefly discussing clinical implications.

A. Similarities and Differences between Forced and Voluntary Abstinence Relapse Models

A question that has guided recent studies has been whether the new voluntary abstinence relapse models would lead to the identification of brain mechanisms of relapse that are distinct from those identified from studies using the classic extinction-reinstatement and home-cage forced abstinence models. The limited literature to date indicates some similarities but also, importantly, some notable differences. Below, we discuss several examples. A methodological caveat in the analysis below is that some comparisons were made across studies in which investigators used different experimental procedures (e.g., different drug unit dose or alcohol concentrations, reinforcement schedules, session durations, and durations of self-administration training and abstinence), which can potentially impact behavior during the relapse tests beyond the impact of the abstinence-inducing manipulation.

1. Punishment and Electric Barrier–Induced Abstinence

In the case of drug priming, injections of both heroin and the anxiolytic drug lorazepam provoke relapse to remifentanil seeking after punishment. In contrast, heroin but not lorazepam priming reinstates remifentanil seeking after extinction (Panlilio et al., 2005). Future studies are needed to determine whether this antipunishment effect of benzodiazepines generalizes to other addictive drugs. However, the selective effect of lorazepam in the punishment-based relapse model extends previous studies on increased operant responding by anxiolytic drugs in the punishment component of the classic Geller-Seifter conflict model (Geller et al., 1962). A potential clinical implication of the lorazepam priming results is that drugs that induce relapse after punishment in the animal model (Panlilio et al., 2005) may provoke relapse in drug users who abstain due to adverse consequences of drug use.

In the case of context-induced reinstatement/relapse after extinction versus punishment, there is evidence for both similarities and differences. Inhibition of LH, vSub, NAc shell, and vSub-to-NAc shell projection decreases context-induced relapse to alcohol seeking after either extinction or punishment (Marchant et al., 2019).

In contrast, inhibition of BLA (but not CeA) increases context-induced relapse of cocaine seeking after punishment, whereas inhibition of either BLA or CeA activity decreases context-induced reinstatement after extinction (Pelloux et al., 2018b). The reasons for the dissociable roles of the amygdala subregions in context-induced relapse after extinction versus punishment are unknown. In both the extinction- and punishment-based ABA renewal models (Marchant et al., 2019; Bouton et al., 2020), drug seeking is induced by exposure to drug-associated contexts and tests occur under extinction conditions. However, an important difference is that extinction-induced abstinence occurs in the absence of the drug, whereas punishment-induced abstinence occurs in the presence of the drug (Marchant et al., 2019). Thus, the two abstinence-induced manipulations involve different forms of new learning: responding does not result in drug delivery versus responding results in drug delivery plus adverse consequences (Marchant et al., 2019). As speculated elsewhere, these and other differences in learning and psychologic processes involved in extinction versus punishment may recruit different brain mechanisms of renewal or context-induced reinstatement/relapse for both drug and nondrug rewards (Marchant et al., 2019; Bouton et al., 2020).

In the case of the electric barrier–induced abstinence model, much less is known about the brain areas controlling relapse in this model. Fredriksson et al. (2021) showed that inhibition of vSub decreases incubated oxycodone seeking after electric barrier–induced abstinence. This finding agrees with previous studies showing that inhibition of vSub decreases context-induced reinstatement of heroin seeking (Bossert and Stern, 2014) after extinction and context-induced relapse of alcohol seeking after punishment (Marchant et al., 2016). Thus, it appears that across drug classes and relapse models, vSub activity is critical for relapse/reinstatement independent of the method to achieve abstinence. However, the lack of effect of vSub inactivation on incubation of oxycodone seeking after forced abstinence suggests a more selective role of vSub in relapse after extinction-, punishment-, or electric barrier–induced abstinence but not forced abstinence.

Finally, Fredriksson et al. (2020) recently showed that incubation of oxycodone seeking is potentiated after electric barrier–induced abstinence compared with home-cage forced abstinence. This finding suggests that the method used to achieve abstinence can also modulate the magnitude of incubation of drug craving (Fig. 3). The data discussed below further support this notion.

2. Food and Social-Choice–Induced Abstinence

Differences in the magnitude and expression of incubation of drug seeking have also been shown in studies using the food and social choice–induced abstinence models. Thus, incubation of heroin seeking is observed after forced abstinence or social choice–induced abstinence but not after food choice–induced abstinence (Venniro et al., 2017b, 2019). Additionally, incubation of methamphetamine seeking is observed after forced abstinence or food choice–induced voluntary abstinence but not after social choice–induced voluntary abstinence (Fig. 3) (Caprioli et al., 2015a; Venniro et al., 2018). As discussed above, dissociable central amygdala mechanisms contribute to the inhibitory effect of social choice on incubation of methamphetamine seeking (CeL PKCδ activity) and to the expression of incubation of drug seeking after home-cage forced abstinence (CeL SOM activity) (Venniro et al., 2020b).

Finally, in the case of similarities in mechanisms between voluntary and forced abstinence, a role of OFC in relapse to opioid seeking has been demonstrated after both forced (Fanous et al., 2012; Altshuler et al., 2021) and food choice–induced abstinence (Reiner et al., 2020).

3. Conclusions

Recent studies indicate some behavioral and neurobiological similarities in relapse after forced versus voluntary abstinence. More importantly, these studies also indicate that the methods used to achieve abstinence can lead to both quantitative (e.g., potentiation of incubation of opioid seeking after electric barrier–induced abstinence) and qualitative (e.g., emergence of incubation of methamphetamine seeking after forced and food choice–induced but not social choice–induced abstinence) differences in relapse-related behaviors. Additionally, the methods used to induce abstinence can lead to recruitment of different neuronal circuits that control inhibition or potentiation of relapse-related behavior (e.g., opposite role of BLA activity in context-induced relapse after extinction versus punishment).

B. Similarities and Differences of Drug Classes within Voluntary Abstinence Models

Studies using the reinstatement and incubation of drug craving after forced-abstinence models have shown both similarities and differences in brain mechanisms of drug seeking across drug classes (Badiani et al., 2011; Bossert et al., 2013). Evidence from recent studies suggests that this is also the case for voluntary abstinence relapse models. Thus, inhibition of vSub decreases both context-induced relapse to alcohol seeking after punishment and incubation of opioid seeking after electric barrier–induced abstinence (Marchant et al., 2016; Fredriksson et al., 2021). Additionally, inhibition of AI decreases relapse to both fentanyl and methamphetamine seeking after food choice–induced abstinence (Venniro et al., 2017a; Reiner et al., 2020). In contrast, inhibition of OFC decreases relapse to fentanyl but not methamphetamine seeking after food choice–induced abstinence (Venniro et al., 2017a; Reiner et al., 2020).

Several behavioral observations also suggest that different brain mechanisms control relapse after voluntary abstinence across drug classes. For example, food choice–induced abstinence prevents the emergence of incubation of heroin or fentanyl seeking but not methamphetamine seeking (Caprioli et al., 2015a; Venniro et al., 2017b; Reiner et al., 2020). Additionally, social choice–induced abstinence prevents the emergence of incubation of methamphetamine seeking and only modestly decreases incubation of heroin seeking (Venniro et al., 2018, 2019). Finally, heroin-trained male rats appear more vulnerable to cue-induced relapse after electric barrier–induced abstinence than cocaine-trained male rats (Peck et al., 2013). One reason for these differences is the different motivational effects of heroin- and cocaine-associated cues during both self-administration and electric barrier suppression. Specifically, we speculate that during the relapse test, the cocaine cues induce higher anxiogenic-like responses that summate with the anxiogenic-like responses induced by the electric barrier, resulting in inhibition of cocaine seeking in most rats. In this regard, elegant studies using the runway model (Ettenberg, 2009) showed that, like food, heroin-reinforced responding is manifested as “pure” approach behavior (decreased run time to the goal box over time). In contrast, like food paired with shock, cocaine-reinforced responding is manifested as approach-avoidance behavior (increased run time to the goal box over time). For additional discussion of these differences and their implications for behavioral and neurobiological mechanisms of cocaine and heroin self-administration and relapse, see Badiani et al. (2011).

In conclusion, results from several studies suggest both similarities and differences in brain mechanisms of relapse after voluntary abstinence across drug classes. However, the profound differences between heroin and methamphetamine in the behavioral effects of food and social choice–induced abstinence on incubation of drug seeking suggest that additional differences in mechanisms of relapse after voluntary abstinence across drug classes are likely to be identified, as has been the case for relapse after forced abstinence or extinction (Badiani et al., 2011; Bossert et al., 2013).

C. Sex as a Biological Variable in Voluntary Abstinence Relapse Models

Over the last several years, we have included both male and female rats in recent studies using voluntary abstinence relapse models. Unexpectedly, under the experimental conditions described in Tables 4, 5, and 7, we found little evidence for sex differences in methamphetamine or opioid (oxycodone and fentanyl) self-administration, voluntary abstinence induced by electric barrier, food choice, or social choice and no evidence for sex differences in relapse/incubation of drug seeking across drug classes (Venniro et al., 2017b, 2019; Fredriksson et al., 2020; Reiner et al., 2020). This contrasts with results from previous studies with cocaine on sex differences and a role of ovarian hormones in drug-priming–induced reinstatement and incubation of drug craving after forced abstinence (Carroll et al., 2004; Kerstetter et al., 2008; Nicolas et al., 2019). The reasons for these differences are unknown but potential reasons might be the different procedures used to achieve abstinence or the different drugs used in the studies (cocaine versus opioids or methamphetamine). Additionally, it should be noted that the experimental conditions described in Tables 4, 5, and 7 were not optimal to determine sex differences in opioid or methamphetamine self-administration and choice because of the use of a single-unit dose of heroin or methamphetamine and choice procedures that biased choice toward the alternative reward and generated low variability within each sex and between sexes. However, these studies were both designed and powered to detect sex differences in incubation of drug craving or electric barrier suppression if such differences exist (Tables 4, 5, and 7).

Nevertheless, these studies provide a strong rationale for including females in relapse-related studies using both forced and voluntary abstinence–based models. This inclusion will allow to both increase the generality of the preclinical results to humans who use drugs and, equally important, to test medications and study neuropharmacological mechanisms in both males and females. In this regard, we recently reported that females are less sensitive than males to the antirelapse effects of potential medications in both the context-induced reinstatement model (Bossert et al., 2020) and the electric barrier–induced abstinence relapse model (Fredriksson et al., 2020).

In conclusion, despite evidence for lack of sex differences for opioid and psychostimulant seeking in the new voluntary abstinence models, we argue that it is critical to include females in such studies. The inclusion of both sexes will improve the generality of the behavioral findings, determine whether there are sex differences in the mechanisms underlying relapse in the new models, and lead to identification of sex-dependent effects of potential medications (Bossert et al., 2020; Fredriksson et al., 2020).

D. Clinical Implications

In the final section of the review, we discuss two potential clinical implications of the voluntary abstinence relapse models. The first implication is for medication development, and the second implication is for the understanding of behavioral and neuropharmacological mechanisms of human drug relapse.

1. Implications for Medication Development

Since the 1990s, numerous studies have used the extinction-reinstatement model to identify novel relapse-prevention medications (Shalev et al., 2002; Spencer and Kalivas, 2017; Reiner et al., 2019). From a translational perspective, the model has shown good postdictive validity with effective human medications (naltrexone, acamprosate, buprenorphine, methadone, or varenicline) decrease reinstatement of drug seeking in the rat model. However, with two exceptions (Kowalczyk et al., 2015; Sinha et al., 2020), the model has yet to show “true” predictive validity (medications identified in the model decrease drug relapse in humans) (Venniro et al., 2020a). Additionally, many “promising” medications identified in the reinstatement model did not decrease drug relapse in humans (Table 2 in Venniro et al. (2020a)).

The weak evidence for the prospective predictive validity of the reinstatement model and similar weak evidence from other animal models (Tables 1 and 3; Venniro et al. (2020a)) have led us and other investigators to develop alternative voluntary abstinence–based relapse models that incorporate critical features of human addiction: abstinence induced by negative consequences of drug use or by the availability of competing alternative nondrug rewards (Marlatt, 2002; Epstein et al., 2006; Ahmed, 2010). A question for future research is whether the voluntary abstinence models will improve the predictive validity of rat relapse models. We hope that this is the case, but it is important to note that improved homology between the animal model and the human condition does not guarantee improved predictive validity (Sarter and Bruno, 2002; Epstein et al., 2006). Therefore, an important initial step in establishing the newer relapse models will be to demonstrate their postdictive validity with FDA-approved medications.

Additionally, although the voluntary abstinence models more closely mimic the human condition than extinction or home-cage abstinence models, they do not fully capture the complex nature of human abstinence. One important difference is that in the rat models, the adverse consequences of drug taking and seeking, or the availability of nondrug rewards, occur in close temporal proximity to the drug-taking or drug-seeking behaviors. In contrast, in humans, both the adverse consequences of using drugs or the availability of nondrug reward when abstaining from drug use are often delayed (Cooper et al., 2007; de Wit et al., 2018).

The voluntary abstinence models were used to identify potential novel pharmacological targets in two studies. In the first study, using male rats, Caprioli et al. (2015a) reported that the mGluR2 (metabotropic glutamate receptor 2) positive allosteric modulator AZD8529 has no effect on nonincubated drug seeking on abstinence day 1 but decreases incubated methamphetamine seeking on abstinence day 21 after forced or food choice–induced voluntary abstinence. These results indicate that the effect of AZD8529 on incubated methamphetamine seeking is independent of the conditions used to achieve abstinence.

In the second study, using both sexes, Fredriksson et al. (2020) reported that in males, the dopamine stabilizer (−)-OSU6162 decreases incubated (but not nonincubated) oxycodone seeking after either electric barrier–induced or forced abstinence. In contrast, in females, (−)-OSU6162 modestly decreased incubated oxycodone seeking after electric barrier–induced abstinence but not forced abstinence. The apparent sex-specific effect of (−)-OSU6162 on incubated oxycodone seeking highlight the importance of testing medications in both males and females.

Together, these results indicate that AZD8529 and (−)-OSU6162 may serve as treatments to prevent relapse in males who use methamphetamine or opioids, respectively. We hope that our review will inspire investigators both to test approved FDA-medications and to identify novel pharmacological targets using the newer voluntary abstinence models. A question for future research is whether the results from the newer relapse models will better translate to humans.

2. Implications for Mechanisms of Human Drug Relapse

A question for future research is whether the use of the newer voluntary abstinence relapse models will result in new insights into behavioral and neuropharmacological mechanisms of drug relapse. Several recent results suggest that this is likely going to be the case. At the behavioral level, perhaps the most important general finding is that, compared with relapse after forced abstinence, relapse after voluntary abstinence induced by adverse consequences is potentiated (Fredriksson et al., 2020), whereas relapse after voluntary abstinence induced by providing alternative food or social reward is inhibited (Fig. 3). This pattern of results from the rat models supports the notion that relapse prevention is more likely to succeed by introducing alternative nondrug rewards than by punishment or incarceration (Hunt and Azrin, 1973; Higgins et al., 1991; Silverman et al., 2012).

At the neuropharmacological level, as discussed above, there is evidence for dissociable abstinence-dependent brain mechanisms of relapse induced by the same relapse-provoking stimulus. Perhaps the most striking example is that BLA activity plays opposite roles in context-induced reinstatement after extinction (inhibition) versus context-induced relapse after punishment (potentiation). These results demonstrate that the amygdala’s role in relapse depends on the method used to achieve abstinence and highlights the importance of studying relapse under different experimental conditions that mimic abstinence conditions that occur in humans who use drugs.

E. Concluding Remarks

In closing, we hope that our review will inspire addiction researchers to incorporate animal models of relapse after voluntary abstinence into their studies. We also hope that more widespread use of these models will improve the predictive validity of the relapse models and our understanding of behavioral and neuropharmacological mechanisms of human drug relapse. However, we do not know whether the use of the newer models will improve the predictive validity of relapse models because, to date, there are no published studies showing postdictive validity of the newer models with FDA-approved medications. And until there is conclusive predictive validity–related evidence that some relapse models are superior to others, we recommend that investigators use a relapse model that best fits their research question.

Authorship Contributions

Wrote or contributed to the writing of the manuscript: Fredriksson, Venniro, Reiner, Chow, Bossert, Shaham.

Footnotes

  • The writing of this review was supported by funds from the Intramural Research Program of National Institutes of Health National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA) [Grant 1ZIADA000434-20] to Y.S., The Swedish Research Council International postdoctoral grant [Grant 2019-00658] to I.F., a grant from NIDA [Grant DA047976] to M.V., and a National Institute of General Medical Sciences Postdoctoral Research Associate Grant [Grant 1F12GM128603] to D.J.R.

  • The authors declare that they do not have any competing interests or conflicts of interest (financial or otherwise) related to the material presented in this manuscript.

  • https://doi.org/10.1124/pharmrev.120.000191.

Abbreviations

AI
anterior insula
AIV
AI cortex ventral
BLA
basolateral amygdala
BNST
bed nucleus of stria terminalis
CART
cocaine and amphetamine-regulated transcript
CeA
central amygdala nucleus
CeL
lateral division of CeA
CeM
medial division of CeA
CTb
cholera toxin B
DLS
dorsolateral striatum
DMS
dorsomedial striatum
DREADD
designer receptor exclusively activated by designer drug
Drd1
dopamine D1 receptor
Drd2
dopamine D2 receptor
DSM-IV
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual IV
F1
filial 1
FDA
Food and Drug Administration
fMRI
functional magnetic resonance imaging
LH
lateral hypothalamus
mPFC
medial prefrontal cortex
NAc
nucleus accumbens
OFC
orbitofrontal cortex
Pir
piriform cortex
PKCδ
protein kinase-Cδ
PVT
paraventricular thalamus
SOM
somatostatin
VP
ventral pallidum
vSub
ventral subiculum
VTA
ventral tegmental area.
  • U.S. Government work not protected by U.S. copyright.

References

    1. Adhikary S
    2. Caprioli D
    3. Venniro M
    4. Kallenberger P
    5. Shaham Y
    6. Bossert JM
    (2017) Incubation of extinction responding and cue-induced reinstatement, but not context- or drug priming-induced reinstatement, after withdrawal from methamphetamine. Addict Biol 22:977–990.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  1. ↵
    1. Ahmed SH
    (2010) Validation crisis in animal models of drug addiction: beyond non-disordered drug use toward drug addiction. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 35:172–184.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Ahmed SH
    (2018) Trying to make sense of rodents’ drug choice behavior. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 87 (Pt A):3–10.
    OpenUrl
  3. ↵
    1. Ahmed SH
    2. Koob GF
    (1998) Transition from moderate to excessive drug intake: change in hedonic set point. Science 282:298–300.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    1. Airavaara M
    2. Pickens CL
    3. Stern AL
    4. Wihbey KA
    5. Harvey BK
    6. Bossert JM
    7. Liu QR
    8. Hoffer BJ
    9. Shaham Y
    (2011) Endogenous GDNF in ventral tegmental area and nucleus accumbens does not play a role in the incubation of heroin craving. Addict Biol 16:261–272.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Altshuler RD
    2. Yang ES
    3. Garcia KT
    4. Davis IR
    5. Olaniran A
    6. Haile M
    7. Razavi S
    8. Li X
    (2021) Role of orbitofrontal cortex in incubation of oxycodone craving in male rats. Addict Biol 26:e12927.
    OpenUrl
  6. ↵
    1. Antelman SM
    2. Levine J
    3. Gershon S
    (2000) Time-dependent sensitization: the odyssey of a scientific heresy from the laboratory to the door of the clinic. Mol Psychiatry 5:350–356.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Honig WK
    1. Azrin NH
    2. Holz WC
    (1966) Punishment, in Operant behavior: areas of research and application (Honig WK, ed) pp 380–447, Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, N.J.
  8. ↵
    1. Badiani A
    2. Belin D
    3. Epstein D
    4. Calu D
    5. Shaham Y
    (2011) Opiate versus psychostimulant addiction: the differences do matter. Nat Rev Neurosci 12:685–700.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Balleine BW
    2. O’Doherty JP
    (2010) Human and rodent homologies in action control: corticostriatal determinants of goal-directed and habitual action. Neuropsychopharmacology 35:48–69.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Banks ML
    2. Negus SS
    (2017) Insights from preclinical choice models on treating drug addiction. Trends Pharmacol Sci 38:181–194.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Barnea-Ygael N
    2. Yadid G
    3. Yaka R
    4. Ben-Shahar O
    5. Zangen A
    (2012) Cue-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking in the rat “conflict model”: effect of prolonged home-cage confinement. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 219:875–883.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Becker JB
    2. Hu M
    (2008) Sex differences in drug abuse. Front Neuroendocrinol 29:36–47.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Bedi G
    2. Preston KL
    3. Epstein DH
    4. Heishman SJ
    5. Marrone GF
    6. Shaham Y
    7. de Wit H
    (2011) Incubation of cue-induced cigarette craving during abstinence in human smokers. Biol Psychiatry 69:708–711.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Bossert JM
    2. Adhikary S
    3. St Laurent R
    4. Marchant NJ
    5. Wang HL
    6. Morales M
    7. Shaham Y
    (2016) Role of projections from ventral subiculum to nucleus accumbens shell in context-induced reinstatement of heroin seeking in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 233:1991–2004.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Bossert JM
    2. Kiyatkin EA
    3. Korah H
    4. Hoots JK
    5. Afzal A
    6. Perekopskiy D
    7. Thomas S
    8. Fredriksson I
    9. Blough BE
    10. Negus SS, et al.
    (2020) In a rat model of opioid maintenance, the G-protein-biased mu opioid receptor agonist TRV130 decreases relapse to oxycodone seeking and taking and prevents oxycodone-induced brain hypoxia. Biol Psychiatry 88:935–944.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Bossert JM
    2. Marchant NJ
    3. Calu DJ
    4. Shaham Y
    (2013) The reinstatement model of drug relapse: recent neurobiological findings, emerging research topics, and translational research. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 229:453–476.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Bossert JM
    2. Stern AL
    (2014) Role of ventral subiculum in context-induced reinstatement of heroin seeking in rats. Addict Biol 19:338–342.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Bossert JM
    2. Stern AL
    3. Theberge FR
    4. Cifani C
    5. Koya E
    6. Hope BT
    7. Shaham Y
    (2011) Ventral medial prefrontal cortex neuronal ensembles mediate context-induced relapse to heroin. Nat Neurosci 14:420–422.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Bouton ME
    2. Bolles RC
    (1979) Contextual control of the extinction of conditioned fear. Learn Motiv 10:445–466.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Bouton ME
    2. Maren S
    3. McNally GP
    (2020) Behavioral and neurobiological mechanisms of Pavlovian and instrumental extinction learning. Physiol Rev 101:611–681.
    OpenUrl
  21. ↵
    1. Bouton ME
    2. Schepers ST
    (2015) Renewal after the punishment of free operant behavior. J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn 41:81–90.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Bouton ME
    2. Swartzentruber D
    (1991) Sources of relapse after extinction in Pavlovian and instrumental learning. Clin Psychol Rev 11:123–140.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  23. ↵
    1. Brandt L
    2. Jones JD
    3. Martinez S
    4. Manubay JM
    5. Mogali S
    6. Ramey T
    7. Levin FR
    8. Comer SD
    (2020) Effects of lorcaserin on oxycodone self-administration and subjective responses in participants with opioid use disorder. Drug Alcohol Depend 208:107859.
    OpenUrl
  24. ↵
    1. Campbell EJ
    2. Flanagan JPM
    3. Marchant NJ
    4. Lawrence AJ
    (2018) Reduced alcohol-seeking in male offspring of sires exposed to alcohol self-administration followed by punishment-imposed abstinence. Pharmacol Res Perspect 6:e00384.
    OpenUrl
  25. ↵
    1. Campbell EJ
    2. Flanagan JPM
    3. Walker LC
    4. Hill MKRI
    5. Marchant NJ
    6. Lawrence AJ
    (2019a) Anterior insular cortex is critical for the propensity to relapse following punishment-imposed abstinence of alcohol seeking. J Neurosci 39:1077–1087.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  26. ↵
    1. Campbell EJ
    2. Jin S
    3. Lawrence AJ
    (2019b) Environmental enrichment reduces the propensity to relapse following punishment-imposed abstinence of alcohol seeking. Physiol Behav 210:112638.
    OpenUrl
  27. ↵
    1. Caprioli D
    2. Venniro M
    3. Zeric T
    4. Li X
    5. Adhikary S
    6. Madangopal R
    7. Marchant NJ
    8. Lucantonio F
    9. Schoenbaum G
    10. Bossert JM, et al.
    (2015a) Effect of the novel positive allosteric modulator of metabotropic glutamate receptor 2 AZD8529 on incubation of methamphetamine craving after prolonged voluntary abstinence in a rat model. Biol Psychiatry 78:463–473.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Caprioli D
    2. Venniro M
    3. Zhang M
    4. Bossert JM
    5. Warren BL
    6. Hope BT
    7. Shaham Y
    (2017) Role of dorsomedial striatum neuronal ensembles in incubation of methamphetamine craving after voluntary abstinence. J Neurosci 37:1014–1027.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  29. ↵
    1. Caprioli D
    2. Zeric T
    3. Thorndike EB
    4. Venniro M
    (2015b) Persistent palatable food preference in rats with a history of limited and extended access to methamphetamine self-administration. Addict Biol 20:913–926.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Carroll ME
    2. Lynch WJ
    3. Roth ME
    4. Morgan AD
    5. Cosgrove KP
    (2004) Sex and estrogen influence drug abuse. Trends Pharmacol Sci 25:273–279.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Cooper A
    2. Barnea-Ygael N
    3. Levy D
    4. Shaham Y
    5. Zangen A
    (2007) A conflict rat model of cue-induced relapse to cocaine seeking. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 194:117–125.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Crombag HS
    2. Shaham Y
    (2002) Renewal of drug seeking by contextual cues after prolonged extinction in rats. Behav Neurosci 116:169–173.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Crombag HS
    2. Bossert JM
    3. Koya E
    4. Shaham Y
    (2008) Review. Context-induced relapse to drug seeking: a review. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 363:3233–3243.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. de Wit H
    (2009) Impulsivity as a determinant and consequence of drug use: a review of underlying processes. Addict Biol 14:22–31.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. de Wit H
    2. Epstein DH
    3. Preston KL
    (2018) Does human language limit translatability of clinical and preclinical addiction research? Neuropsychopharmacology 43:1985–1988.
    OpenUrl
  36. ↵
    1. Deroche-Gamonet V
    2. Belin D
    3. Piazza PV
    (2004) Evidence for addiction-like behavior in the rat. Science 305:1014–1017.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  37. ↵
    1. Dong Y
    2. Taylor JR
    3. Wolf ME
    4. Shaham Y
    (2017) Circuit and synaptic plasticity mechanisms of drug relapse. J Neurosci 37:10867–10876.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  38. ↵
    1. Economidou D
    2. Pelloux Y
    3. Robbins TW
    4. Dalley JW
    5. Everitt BJ
    (2009) High impulsivity predicts relapse to cocaine-seeking after punishment-induced abstinence. Biol Psychiatry 65:851–856.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Epstein DH
    2. Preston KL
    (2003) The reinstatement model and relapse prevention: a clinical perspective. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 168:31–41.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Epstein DH
    2. Preston KL
    3. Stewart J
    4. Shaham Y
    (2006) Toward a model of drug relapse: an assessment of the validity of the reinstatement procedure. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 189:1–16.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Ettenberg A
    (2009) The runway model of drug self-administration. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 91:271–277.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Ewing ST
    2. Dorcely C
    3. Maidi R
    4. Paker G
    5. Schelbaum E
    6. Ranaldi R
    (2021) Low-dose polypharmacology targeting dopamine D1 and D3 receptors reduces cue-induced relapse to heroin seeking in rats. Addict Biol DOI: 10.1111/adb.12988 [published ahead of print].
  43. ↵
    1. Fanous S
    2. Goldart EM
    3. Theberge FR
    4. Bossert JM
    5. Shaham Y
    6. Hope BT
    (2012) Role of orbitofrontal cortex neuronal ensembles in the expression of incubation of heroin craving. J Neurosci 32:11600–11609.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  44. ↵
    1. Farrell MR
    2. Ruiz CM
    3. Castillo E
    4. Faget L
    5. Khanbijian C
    6. Liu S
    7. Schoch H
    8. Rojas G
    9. Huerta MY
    10. Hnasko TS, et al.
    (2019) Ventral pallidum is essential for cocaine relapse after voluntary abstinence in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 44:2174–2185.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    1. Fredriksson I
    2. Shekara A
    3. Applebey SV
    4. Minier-Toribio S
    5. Altidor L
    6. Cifani C
    7. Hope BT
    8. Bossert JM
    9. Shaham Y (2021)
    Role of ventral subiculum neuronal ensembles in incubation of oxycodone craving after electric barrier-induced voluntary abstinence. bioRxiv. doi:10.1101/2021.03.24.436801
  46. ↵
    1. Fredriksson I
    2. Applebey SV
    3. Minier-Toribio A
    4. Shekara A
    5. Bossert JM
    6. Shaham Y
    (2020) Effect of the dopamine stabilizer (-)-OSU6162 on potentiated incubation of opioid craving after electric barrier-induced voluntary abstinence. Neuropsychopharmacology 45:770–779.
    OpenUrl
  47. ↵
    1. Fuchs RA
    2. Evans KA
    3. Ledford CC
    4. Parker MP
    5. Case JM
    6. Mehta RH
    7. See RE
    (2005) The role of the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, basolateral amygdala, and dorsal hippocampus in contextual reinstatement of cocaine seeking in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 30:296–309.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. Gasior M
    2. Paronis CA
    3. Bergman J
    (2004) Modification by dopaminergic drugs of choice behavior under concurrent schedules of intravenous saline and food delivery in monkeys. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 308:249–259.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  49. ↵
    1. Geller I
    2. Kulak JT Jr.
    3. Seifter J
    (1962) The effects of chlordiazepoxide and chlorpromazine on a punishment discrimination. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 3:374–385.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    1. Glynn RM
    2. Rosenkranz JA
    3. Wolf ME
    4. Caccamise A
    5. Shroff F
    6. Smith AB
    7. Loweth JA
    (2018) Repeated restraint stress exposure during early withdrawal accelerates incubation of cue-induced cocaine craving. Addict Biol 23:80–89.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    1. Grimm JW
    2. Hope BT
    3. Wise RA
    4. Shaham Y
    (2001) Neuroadaptation. Incubation of cocaine craving after withdrawal. Nature 412:141–142.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Grimm JW
    2. Lu L
    3. Hayashi T
    4. Hope BT
    5. Su TP
    6. Shaham Y
    (2003) Time-dependent increases in brain-derived neurotrophic factor protein levels within the mesolimbic dopamine system after withdrawal from cocaine: implications for incubation of cocaine craving. J Neurosci 23:742–747.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  52. ↵
    1. Grimm JW
    (2020) Incubation of food craving in rats: A review. J Exp Anal Behav 113:37–47.
    OpenUrl
  53. ↵
    1. Groenewegen HJ
    2. Vermeulen-Van der Zee E
    3. te Kortschot A
    4. Witter MP
    (1987) Organization of the projections from the subiculum to the ventral striatum in the rat. A study using anterograde transport of Phaseolus vulgaris leucoagglutinin. Neuroscience 23:103–120.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    1. Heilig M
    2. Epstein DH
    3. Nader MA
    4. Shaham Y
    (2016) Time to connect: bringing social context into addiction neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci 17:592–599.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  55. ↵
    1. Heyman GM
    (2013) Addiction and choice: theory and new data. Front Psychiatry 4:31.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    1. Higgins ST
    2. Delaney DD
    3. Budney AJ
    4. Bickel WK
    5. Hughes JR
    6. Foerg F
    7. Fenwick JW
    (1991) A behavioral approach to achieving initial cocaine abstinence. Am J Psychiatry 148:1218–1224.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    1. Higgins ST
    2. Heil SH
    3. Lussier JP
    (2004) Clinical implications of reinforcement as a determinant of substance use disorders. Annu Rev Psychol 55:431–461.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    1. Hu Y
    2. Salmeron BJ
    3. Krasnova IN
    4. Gu H
    5. Lu H
    6. Bonci A
    7. Cadet JL
    8. Stein EA
    9. Yang Y
    (2019) Compulsive drug use is associated with imbalance of orbitofrontal- and prelimbic-striatal circuits in punishment-resistant individuals. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 116:9066–9071.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  59. ↵
    1. Hunt GM
    2. Azrin NH
    (1973) A community-reinforcement approach to alcoholism. Behav Res Ther 11:91–104.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  60. ↵
    1. Jenkins TN
    2. Warner LH
    3. Warden CJ
    (1926) Standard apparatus for the study of animal motivation. J Comp Psychol 6:361–382.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  61. ↵
    1. Justinova Z
    2. Panlilio LV
    3. Secci ME
    4. Redhi GH
    5. Schindler CW
    6. Cross AJ
    7. Mrzljak L
    8. Medd A
    9. Shaham Y
    10. Goldberg SR
    (2015) The novel metabotropic glutamate receptor 2 positive allosteric modulator, AZD8529, decreases nicotine self-administration and relapse in squirrel monkeys. Biol Psychiatry 78:452–462.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  62. ↵
    1. Kalivas PW
    2. Stewart J
    (1991) Dopamine transmission in the initiation and expression of drug- and stress-induced sensitization of motor activity. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 16:223–244.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  63. ↵
    1. Kerstetter KA
    2. Aguilar VR
    3. Parrish AB
    4. Kippin TE
    (2008) Protracted time-dependent increases in cocaine-seeking behavior during cocaine withdrawal in female relative to male rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 198:63–75.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    1. Khemiri L
    2. Steensland P
    3. Guterstam J
    4. Beck O
    5. Carlsson A
    6. Franck J
    7. Jayaram-Lindström N
    (2015) The effects of the monoamine stabilizer (-)-OSU6162 on craving in alcohol dependent individuals: a human laboratory study. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 25:2240–2251.
    OpenUrl
  65. ↵
    1. Kowalczyk WJ
    2. Phillips KA
    3. Jobes ML
    4. Kennedy AP
    5. Ghitza UE
    6. Agage DA
    7. Schmittner JP
    8. Epstein DH
    9. Preston KL
    (2015) Clonidine maintenance prolongs opioid abstinence and decouples stress from craving in daily life: a randomized controlled trial with ecological momentary assessment. Am J Psychiatry 172:760–767.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  66. ↵
    1. Koya E
    2. Golden SA
    3. Harvey BK
    4. Guez-Barber DH
    5. Berkow A
    6. Simmons DE
    7. Bossert JM
    8. Nair SG
    9. Uejima JL
    10. Marin MT, et al.
    (2009) Targeted disruption of cocaine-activated nucleus accumbens neurons prevents context-specific sensitization. Nat Neurosci 12:1069–1073.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  67. ↵
    1. Krasnova IN
    2. Gerra MC
    3. Walther D
    4. Jayanthi S
    5. Ladenheim B
    6. McCoy MT
    7. Brannock C
    8. Cadet JL
    (2017) Compulsive methamphetamine taking in the presence of punishment is associated with increased oxytocin expression in the nucleus accumbens of rats. Sci Rep 7:8331.
    OpenUrl
  68. ↵
    1. Krasnova IN
    2. Marchant NJ
    3. Ladenheim B
    4. McCoy MT
    5. Panlilio LV
    6. Bossert JM
    7. Shaham Y
    8. Cadet JL
    (2014) Incubation of methamphetamine and palatable food craving after punishment-induced abstinence. Neuropsychopharmacology 39:2008–2016.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  69. ↵
    1. Kruzich PJ
    2. Congleton KM
    3. See RE
    (2001) Conditioned reinstatement of drug-seeking behavior with a discrete compound stimulus classically conditioned with intravenous cocaine. Behav Neurosci 115:1086–1092.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  70. ↵
    1. Kwako LE
    2. Spagnolo PA
    3. Schwandt ML
    4. Thorsell A
    5. George DT
    6. Momenan R
    7. Rio DE
    8. Huestis M
    9. Anizan S
    10. Concheiro M, et al.
    (2015) The corticotropin releasing hormone-1 (CRH1) receptor antagonist pexacerfont in alcohol dependence: a randomized controlled experimental medicine study. Neuropsychopharmacology 40:1053–1063.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  71. ↵
    1. LaRowe SD
    2. Kalivas PW
    3. Nicholas JS
    4. Randall PK
    5. Mardikian PN
    6. Malcolm RJ
    (2013) A double-blind placebo-controlled trial of N-acetylcysteine in the treatment of cocaine dependence. Am J Addict 22:443–452.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  72. ↵
    1. Lenoir M
    2. Cantin L
    3. Vanhille N
    4. Serre F
    5. Ahmed SH
    (2013) Extended heroin access increases heroin choices over a potent nondrug alternative. Neuropsychopharmacology 38:1209–1220.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  73. ↵
    1. Lenoir M
    2. Serre F
    3. Cantin L
    4. Ahmed SH
    (2007) Intense sweetness surpasses cocaine reward. PLoS One 2:e698.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  74. ↵
    1. Li P
    2. Wu P
    3. Xin X
    4. Fan YL
    5. Wang GB
    6. Wang F
    7. Ma MY
    8. Xue MM
    9. Luo YX
    10. Yang FD, et al.
    (2015a) Incubation of alcohol craving during abstinence in patients with alcohol dependence. Addict Biol 20:513–522.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  75. ↵
    1. Li X
    2. Rubio FJ
    3. Zeric T
    4. Bossert JM
    5. Kambhampati S
    6. Cates HM
    7. Kennedy PJ
    8. Liu QR
    9. Cimbro R
    10. Hope BT, et al.
    (2015b) Incubation of methamphetamine craving is associated with selective increases in expression of Bdnf and trkb, glutamate receptors, and epigenetic enzymes in cue-activated fos-expressing dorsal striatal neurons. J Neurosci 35:8232–8244.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  76. ↵
    1. Li X
    2. Witonsky KR
    3. Lofaro OM
    4. Surjono F
    5. Zhang J
    6. Bossert JM
    7. Shaham Y
    (2018) Role of anterior intralaminar nuclei of thalamus projections to dorsomedial striatum in incubation of methamphetamine craving. J Neurosci 38:2270–2282.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  77. ↵
    1. Lu L
    2. Grimm JW
    3. Hope BT
    4. Shaham Y
    (2004) Incubation of cocaine craving after withdrawal: a review of preclinical data. Neuropharmacology 47 (Suppl 1):214–226.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  78. ↵
    1. Mantsch JR
    2. Baker DA
    3. Funk D
    4. Lê AD
    5. Shaham Y
    (2016) Stress-induced reinstatement of drug seeking: 20 years of progress. Neuropsychopharmacology 41:335–356.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  79. ↵
    1. Marchant NJ
    2. Campbell EJ
    3. Pelloux Y
    4. Bossert JM
    5. Shaham Y
    (2019) Context-induced relapse after extinction versus punishment: similarities and differences. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 236:439–448.
    OpenUrl
  80. ↵
    1. Marchant NJ
    2. Campbell EJ
    3. Whitaker LR
    4. Harvey BK
    5. Kaganovsky K
    6. Adhikary S
    7. Hope BT
    8. Heins RC
    9. Prisinzano TE
    10. Vardy E, et al.
    (2016) Role of ventral subiculum in context-induced relapse to alcohol seeking after punishment-imposed abstinence. J Neurosci 36:3281–3294.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  81. ↵
    1. Marchant NJ
    2. Kaganovsky K
    (2015) A critical role of nucleus accumbens dopamine D1-family receptors in renewal of alcohol seeking after punishment-imposed abstinence. Behav Neurosci 129:281–291.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  82. ↵
    1. Marchant NJ
    2. Khuc TN
    3. Pickens CL
    4. Bonci A
    5. Shaham Y
    (2013a) Context-induced relapse to alcohol seeking after punishment in a rat model. Biol Psychiatry 73:256–262.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  83. ↵
    1. Marchant NJ
    2. Li X
    3. Shaham Y
    (2013b) Recent developments in animal models of drug relapse. Curr Opin Neurobiol 23:675–683.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  84. ↵
    1. Marchant NJ
    2. Rabei R
    3. Kaganovsky K
    4. Caprioli D
    5. Bossert JM
    6. Bonci A
    7. Shaham Y
    (2014) A critical role of lateral hypothalamus in context-induced relapse to alcohol seeking after punishment-imposed abstinence. J Neurosci 34:7447–7457.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  85. ↵
    1. Marlatt AG
    (1996) Models of relapse and relapse prevention: a commentary. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 4:55–60.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  86. ↵
    1. Marlatt GA
    (2002) Do animal models provide a valid analogue for human drug lapse and relapse? Comment on Leri and Stewart (2002). Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 10:359–360, discussion 364–366.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  87. ↵
    1. McEwen BS
    2. Nasca C
    3. Gray JD
    (2016) Stress effects on neuronal structure: Hippocampus, amygdala, and prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology 41:3–23.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  88. ↵
    1. McFarland K
    2. Kalivas PW
    (2001) The circuitry mediating cocaine-induced reinstatement of drug-seeking behavior. J Neurosci 21:8655–8663.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  89. ↵
    1. Morgan JI
    2. Curran T
    (1991) Stimulus-transcription coupling in the nervous system: involvement of the inducible proto-oncogenes fos and jun. Annu Rev Neurosci 14:421–451.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  90. ↵
    1. Nader MA
    2. Banks ML
    (2014) Environmental modulation of drug taking: Nonhuman primate models of cocaine abuse and PET neuroimaging. Neuropharmacology 76 (Pt B):510-517.
    OpenUrl
  91. ↵
    1. Natesan S
    2. Svensson KA
    3. Reckless GE
    4. Nobrega JN
    5. Barlow KB
    6. Johansson AM
    7. Kapur S
    (2006) The dopamine stabilizers (S)-(-)-(3-methanesulfonyl-phenyl)-1-propyl-piperidine [(-)-OSU6162] and 4-(3-methanesulfonylphenyl)-1-propyl-piperidine (ACR16) show high in vivo D2 receptor occupancy, antipsychotic-like efficacy, and low potential for motor side effects in the rat. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 318:810–818.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  92. ↵
    1. Nauta WJ
    2. Smith GP
    3. Faull RL
    4. Domesick VB
    (1978) Efferent connections and nigral afferents of the nucleus accumbens septi in the rat. Neuroscience 3:385–401.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Neisewander JL
    2. Baker DA
    3. Fuchs RA
    4. Tran-Nguyen LT
    5. Palmer A
    6. Marshall JF
    (2000) Fos protein expression and cocaine-seeking behavior in rats after exposure to a cocaine self-administration environment. J Neurosci 20:798–805.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  93. ↵
    1. Nestler EJ
    2. Aghajanian GK
    (1997) Molecular and cellular basis of addiction. Science 278:58–63.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  94. ↵
    1. Nicolas C
    2. Russell TI
    3. Pierce AF
    4. Maldera S
    5. Holley A
    6. You ZB
    7. McCarthy MM
    8. Shaham Y
    9. Ikemoto S
    (2019) Incubation of cocaine craving after intermittent-access self-administration: sex differences and estrous cycle. Biol Psychiatry 85:915–924.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  95. ↵
    1. O’Brien CP
    2. Childress AR
    3. McLellan AT
    4. Ehrman R
    (1992) Classical conditioning in drug-dependent humans. Ann N Y Acad Sci 654:400–415.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  96. ↵
    1. O’Donnell P
    (2003) Dopamine gating of forebrain neural ensembles. Eur J Neurosci 17:429–435.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  97. ↵
    1. Panlilio LV
    2. Thorndike EB
    3. Schindler CW
    (2003) Reinstatement of punishment-suppressed opioid self-administration in rats: an alternative model of relapse to drug abuse. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 168:229–235.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  98. ↵
    1. Panlilio LV
    2. Thorndike EB
    3. Schindler CW
    (2005) Lorazepam reinstates punishment-suppressed remifentanil self-administration in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 179:374–382.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  99. ↵
    1. Parkes SL
    2. Bradfield LA
    3. Balleine BW
    (2015) Interaction of insular cortex and ventral striatum mediates the effect of incentive memory on choice between goal-directed actions. J Neurosci 35:6464–6471.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  100. ↵
    1. Parvaz MA
    2. Moeller SJ
    3. Goldstein RZ
    (2016) Incubation of cue-induced craving in adults addicted to cocaine measured by electroencephalography. JAMA Psychiatry 73:1127–1134.
    OpenUrl
  101. ↵
    1. Peck JA
    2. Wercberger R
    3. Kariyeva E
    4. Ranaldi R
    (2013) Cue-induced resumption of heroin and cocaine seeking in rats using a conflict model of abstinence and relapse. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 228:651–658.
    OpenUrl
  102. ↵
    1. Pelloux Y
    2. Everitt BJ
    3. Dickinson A
    (2007) Compulsive drug seeking by rats under punishment: effects of drug taking history. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 194:127–137.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  103. ↵
    1. Pelloux Y
    2. Hoots JK
    3. Cifani C
    4. Adhikary S
    5. Martin J
    6. Minier-Toribio A
    7. Bossert JM
    8. Shaham Y
    (2018a) Context-induced relapse to cocaine seeking after punishment-imposed abstinence is associated with activation of cortical and subcortical brain regions. Addict Biol 23:699–712.
    OpenUrl
  104. ↵
    1. Pelloux Y
    2. Minier-Toribio A
    3. Hoots JK
    4. Bossert JM
    5. Shaham Y
    (2018b) Opposite effects of basolateral amygdala inactivation on context-induced relapse to cocaine seeking after extinction versus punishment. J Neurosci 38:51–59.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  105. ↵
    1. Pelloux Y
    2. Murray JE
    3. Everitt BJ
    (2013) Differential roles of the prefrontal cortical subregions and basolateral amygdala in compulsive cocaine seeking and relapse after voluntary abstinence in rats. Eur J Neurosci 38:3018–3026.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  106. ↵
    1. Penn PE
    2. McBride WJ
    3. Lumeng L
    4. Gaff TM
    5. Li TK
    (1978) Neurochemical and operant behavioral studies of a strain of alcohol-preferring rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 8:475–481.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  107. ↵
    1. Peterson GB
    2. Ackilt JE
    3. Frommer GP
    4. Hearst ES
    (1972) Conditioned approach and contact behavior toward signals for food or brain-stimulation reinforcement. Science 177:1009–1011.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  108. ↵
    1. Pickens CL
    2. Airavaara M
    3. Theberge F
    4. Fanous S
    5. Hope BT
    6. Shaham Y
    (2011) Neurobiology of the incubation of drug craving. Trends Neurosci 34:411–420.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  109. ↵
    1. Pickens CL
    2. Golden SA
    3. Adams-Deutsch T
    4. Nair SG
    5. Shaham Y
    (2009) Long-lasting incubation of conditioned fear in rats. Biol Psychiatry 65:881–886.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  110. ↵
    1. Preston KL
    2. Umbricht A
    3. Epstein DH
    (2002) Abstinence reinforcement maintenance contingency and one-year follow-up. Drug Alcohol Depend 67:125–137.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  111. ↵
    1. Reiner DJ
    2. Fredriksson I
    3. Lofaro OM
    4. Bossert JM
    5. Shaham Y
    (2019) Relapse to opioid seeking in rat models: behavior, pharmacology and circuits. Neuropsychopharmacology 44:465–477.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  112. ↵
    1. Reiner DJ
    2. Lofaro OM
    3. Applebey SV
    4. Korah H
    5. Venniro M
    6. Cifani C
    7. Bossert JM
    8. Shaham Y
    (2020) Role of projections between piriform cortex and orbitofrontal cortex in relapse to fentanyl seeking after palatable food choice-induced voluntary abstinence. J Neurosci 40:2485–2497.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  113. ↵
    1. Robinson TE
    2. Becker JB
    (1986) Enduring changes in brain and behavior produced by chronic amphetamine administration: a review and evaluation of animal models of amphetamine psychosis. Brain Res 396:157–198.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  114. ↵
    1. Rossi LM
    2. Reverte I
    3. Ragozzino D
    4. Badiani A
    5. Venniro M
    6. Caprioli D
    (2020) Role of nucleus accumbens core but not shell in incubation of methamphetamine craving after voluntary abstinence. Neuropsychopharmacology 45:256–265.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  115. ↵
    1. Roth ME
    2. Cosgrove KP
    3. Carroll ME
    (2004) Sex differences in the vulnerability to drug abuse: a review of preclinical studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 28:533–546.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  116. ↵
    1. Rothermel M
    2. Brunert D
    3. Zabawa C
    4. Díaz-Quesada M
    5. Wachowiak M
    (2013) Transgene expression in target-defined neuron populations mediated by retrograde infection with adeno-associated viral vectors. J Neurosci 33:15195–15206.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  117. ↵
    1. Rung JP
    2. Rung E
    3. Helgeson L
    4. Johansson AM
    5. Svensson K
    6. Carlsson A
    7. Carlsson ML
    (2008) Effects of (-)-OSU6162 and ACR16 on motor activity in rats, indicating a unique mechanism of dopaminergic stabilization. J Neural Transm (Vienna) 115:899–908.
    OpenUrl
  118. ↵
    1. D’haenen H
    2. den Boer JA
    3. Willner P
    1. Sarter M
    2. Bruno JP
    (2002) Animal models in biological psychiatry, in Biolotical psychiatry (D’haenen H den Boer JA Willner P, eds) pp 1–8, Johns Willey & Sons Ltd., Hoboken, NJ.
  119. ↵
    1. Saunders BT
    2. Robinson TE
    (2010) A cocaine cue acts as an incentive stimulus in some but not others: implications for addiction. Biol Psychiatry 67:730–736.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  120. ↵
    1. Saunders BT
    2. Yager LM
    3. Robinson TE
    (2013) Cue-evoked cocaine “craving”: role of dopamine in the accumbens core. J Neurosci 33:13989–14000.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  121. ↵
    1. Schwandt ML
    2. Cortes CR
    3. Kwako LE
    4. George DT
    5. Momenan R
    6. Sinha R
    7. Grigoriadis DE
    8. Pich EM
    9. Leggio L
    10. Heilig M
    (2016) The CRF1 antagonist verucerfont in anxious alcohol-dependent women: Translation of neuroendocrine, but not of anti-craving effects. Neuropsychopharmacology 41:2818–2829.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  122. ↵
    1. See RE
    (2002) Neural substrates of conditioned-cued relapse to drug-seeking behavior. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 71:517–529.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  123. ↵
    1. See RE
    2. Grimm JW
    3. Kruzich PJ
    4. Rustay N
    (1999) The importance of a compound stimulus in conditioned drug-seeking behavior following one week of extinction from self-administered cocaine in rats. Drug Alcohol Depend 57:41–49.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  124. ↵
    1. Shaham Y
    2. Adamson LK
    3. Grocki S
    4. Corrigall WA
    (1997) Reinstatement and spontaneous recovery of nicotine seeking in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 130:396–403.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  125. ↵
    1. Shaham Y
    2. Shalev U
    3. Lu L
    4. de Wit H
    5. Stewart J
    (2003) The reinstatement model of drug relapse: history, methodology and major findings. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 168:3–20.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  126. ↵
    1. Shalev U
    2. Grimm JW
    3. Shaham Y
    (2002) Neurobiology of relapse to heroin and cocaine seeking: a review. Pharmacol Rev 54:1–42.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  127. ↵
    1. Shalev U
    2. Morales M
    3. Hope B
    4. Yap J
    5. Shaham Y
    (2001) Time-dependent changes in extinction behavior and stress-induced reinstatement of drug seeking following withdrawal from heroin in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 156:98–107.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  128. ↵
    1. Silverman K
    2. DeFulio A
    3. Sigurdsson SO
    (2012) Maintenance of reinforcement to address the chronic nature of drug addiction. Prev Med 55 (Suppl):S46–S53.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  129. ↵
    1. Simpson TL
    2. Saxon AJ
    3. Stappenbeck C
    4. Malte CA
    5. Lyons R
    6. Tell D
    7. Millard SP
    8. Raskind M
    (2018) Double-blind randomized clinical trial of prazosin for alcohol use disorder. Am J Psychiatry 175:1216–1224.
    OpenUrl
  130. ↵
    1. Sinha R
    (2011) New findings on biological factors predicting addiction relapse vulnerability. Curr Psychiatry Rep 13:398–405.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  131. ↵
    1. Sinha R
    2. Shaham Y
    3. Heilig M
    (2011) Translational and reverse translational research on the role of stress in drug craving and relapse. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 218:69–82.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  132. ↵
    1. Sinha R
    2. Wemm S
    3. Milivojevic D
    4. Fogelman N
    5. Morgan PT
    6. Angarita GA
    7. Hermes G
    8. Fox HC
    (2020) Alcohol withdrawal symptoms moderate prazosin’s efficacy in the treatment of alcohol use disorder. Am J Psychiatry (in press) Am J Psychiatry.
  133. ↵
    1. Sonesson C
    2. Lin CH
    3. Hansson L
    4. Waters N
    5. Svensson K
    6. Carlsson A
    7. Smith MW
    8. Wikström H
    (1994) Substituted (S)-phenylpiperidines and rigid congeners as preferential dopamine autoreceptor antagonists: synthesis and structure-activity relationships. J Med Chem 37:2735–2753.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  134. ↵
    1. Spencer S
    2. Kalivas PW
    (2017) Glutamate transport: A new bench to bedside mechanism for treating drug abuse. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 20:797–812.
    OpenUrl
  135. ↵
    1. Bozarth MA
    1. Stewart J
    2. de Wit H
    (1987) Reinstatement of drug-taking behavior as a method of assessing incentive motivational properties of drugs, in Methods of assessing the reinforcing properties of abused drugs (Bozarth MA, ed) pp 211–227, Springer-Verlag, New York.
  136. ↵
    1. Cox WM
    2. Klinger E
    1. Stitzer ML
    2. Jones HE
    3. Tuten M
    4. Wong C
    (2011) Community reinforcement approach and contingency management interventions for substance abuse Handbook of Motivational Counseling: Goal-Based Approaches to Assessment and Intervention with Addiction and Other Problems (Cox WM Klinger E, eds John Wiley & Sons, Ltd, Chichester, UK.
    1. Szumlinski KK
    2. Shin CB
    (2018) Kinase interest you in treating incubated cocaine-craving? A hypothetical model for treatment intervention during protracted withdrawal from cocaine. Genes Brain Behav 17:e12440.
    OpenUrl
  137. ↵
    1. Torres OV
    2. Jayanthi S
    3. Ladenheim B
    4. McCoy MT
    5. Krasnova IN
    6. Cadet JL
    (2017) Compulsive methamphetamine taking under punishment is associated with greater cue-induced drug seeking in rats. Behav Brain Res 326:265–271.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Tran-Nguyen TL
    2. Fuchs RA
    3. Coffey GP
    4. O’Dell LE
    5. Baker DA
    6. Neisewander JL
    (1998) Time-dependent changes in cocaine-seeking behavior and dopamine overflow levels in the amygdala during cocaine withdrawal. Neuropsychopharmacology 19:48–59.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  138. ↵
    1. Vanderschuren LJM
    2. Minnaard AM
    3. Smeets JAS
    4. Lesscher HMB
    (2017) Punishment models of addictive behavior. Curr Opin Behav Sci 13:77–84.
    OpenUrl
  139. ↵
    1. Venniro M
    2. Banks ML
    3. Heilig M
    4. Epstein DH
    5. Shaham Y
    (2020a) Improving translation of animal models of addiction and relapse by reverse translation. Nat Rev Neurosci 21:625–643.
    OpenUrl
  140. ↵
    1. Venniro M
    2. Caprioli D
    3. Shaham Y
    (2016) Animal models of drug relapse and craving: From drug priming-induced reinstatement to incubation of craving after voluntary abstinence. Prog Brain Res 224:25–52.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  141. ↵
    1. Venniro M
    2. Caprioli D
    3. Zhang M
    4. Whitaker LR
    5. Zhang S
    6. Warren BL
    7. Cifani C
    8. Marchant NJ
    9. Yizhar O
    10. Bossert JM, et al.
    (2017a) The anterior insular cortex→central amygdala glutamatergic pathway is critical to relapse after contingency management. Neuron 96:414–427.e8.
    OpenUrl
  142. ↵
    1. Venniro M
    2. Russell TI
    3. Ramsey LA
    4. Richie CT
    5. Lesscher HMB
    6. Giovanetti SM
    7. Messing RO
    8. Shaham Y
    (2020b) Abstinence-dependent dissociable central amygdala microcircuits control drug craving. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 117:8126–8134.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  143. ↵
    1. Venniro M
    2. Russell TI
    3. Zhang M
    4. Shaham Y
    (2019) Operant social reward decreases incubation of heroin craving in male and female rats. Biol Psychiatry 86:848–856.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  144. ↵
    1. Venniro M
    2. Shaham Y
    (2020) An operant social self-administration and choice model in rats. Nat Protoc 15:1542–1559.
    OpenUrl
  145. ↵
    1. Venniro M
    2. Zhang M
    3. Caprioli D
    4. Hoots JK
    5. Golden SA
    6. Heins C
    7. Morales M
    8. Epstein DH
    9. Shaham Y
    (2018) Volitional social interaction prevents drug addiction in rat models. Nat Neurosci 21:1520–1529.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  146. ↵
    1. Venniro M
    2. Zhang M
    3. Shaham Y
    4. Caprioli D
    (2017b) Incubation of methamphetamine but not heroin craving after voluntary abstinence in male and female rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 42:1126–1135.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  147. ↵
    1. Wang G
    2. Shi J
    3. Chen N
    4. Xu L
    5. Li J
    6. Li P
    7. Sun Y
    8. Lu L
    (2013) Effects of length of abstinence on decision-making and craving in methamphetamine abusers. PLoS One 8:e68791.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  148. ↵
    1. Warden CJ
    (1931) Animal motivation: experimental studies on the albino rat, Columbia University Press, New York.
  149. ↵
    1. Wikler A
    (1973) Dynamics of drug dependence. Implications of a conditioning theory for research and treatment. Arch Gen Psychiatry 28:611–616.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  150. ↵
    1. Winhusen TM
    2. Kropp F
    3. Lindblad R
    4. Douaihy A
    5. Haynes L
    6. Hodgkins C
    7. Chartier K
    8. Kampman KM
    9. Sharma G
    10. Lewis DF, et al.
    (2014) Multisite, randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled pilot clinical trial to evaluate the efficacy of buspirone as a relapse-prevention treatment for cocaine dependence. J Clin Psychiatry 75:757–764.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  151. ↵
    1. Wise RA
    (1973) Voluntary ethanol intake in rats following exposure to ethanol on various schedules. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 29:203–210.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  152. ↵
    1. Wolf ME
    (2016) Synaptic mechanisms underlying persistent cocaine craving. Nat Rev Neurosci 17:351–365.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Zimmer BA
    2. Oleson EB
    3. Roberts DC
    (2012) The motivation to self-administer is increased after a history of spiking brain levels of cocaine. Neuropsychopharmacology 37:1901–1910.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
PreviousNext
Back to top

In this issue

Pharmacological Reviews: 73 (3)
Pharmacological Reviews
Vol. 73, Issue 3
1 Jul 2021
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Editorial Board (PDF)
  • Front Matter (PDF)
Download PDF
Article Alerts
Sign In to Email Alerts with your Email Address
Email Article

Thank you for sharing this Pharmacological Reviews article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Animal Models of Drug Relapse and Craving after Voluntary Abstinence: A Review
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Pharmacological Reviews
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Pharmacological Reviews.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Citation Tools
Review ArticleReview Article

Relapse after Voluntary Abstinence

Ida Fredriksson, Marco Venniro, David J. Reiner, Jonathan J. Chow, Jennifer M. Bossert and Yavin Shaham
Pharmacological Reviews July 1, 2021, 73 (3) 1050-1083; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1124/pharmrev.120.000191

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero

Share
Review ArticleReview Article

Relapse after Voluntary Abstinence

Ida Fredriksson, Marco Venniro, David J. Reiner, Jonathan J. Chow, Jennifer M. Bossert and Yavin Shaham
Pharmacological Reviews July 1, 2021, 73 (3) 1050-1083; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1124/pharmrev.120.000191
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • I. Introduction
    • II. Relapse after Abstinence Induced by Adverse Consequences of Drug Taking and Seeking
    • III. Relapse after Abstinence Induced by Availability of Alternative Nondrug Rewards
    • IV. Conclusions and Clinical Implications
    • Authorship Contributions
    • Footnotes
    • Abbreviations
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

  • ABCB1 and ABCG2 Regulation at the Blood-Brain Barrier
  • Review of Natural Language Processing in Pharmacology
  • PHARMACOGENOMICS: Driving Personalized Medicine
Show more Review article

Similar Articles

Advertisement
  • Home
  • Alerts
Facebook   Twitter   LinkedIn   RSS

Navigate

  • Current Issue
  • Latest Articles
  • Archive
  • Search for Articles
  • Feedback
  • ASPET

More Information

  • About Pharmacological Reviews
  • Editorial Board
  • Instructions to Authors
  • Submit a Manuscript
  • Customized Alerts
  • RSS Feeds
  • Subscriptions
  • Permissions
  • Terms & Conditions of Use

ASPET's Other Journals

  • Drug Metabolism and Disposition
  • Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics
  • Molecular Pharmacology
  • Pharmacology Research & Perspectives
ISSN 1521-0081 (Online)

Copyright © 2023 by the American Society for Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics