Abstract
A widely held dogma in the preclinical addiction field is that females are more vulnerable than males to drug craving and relapse. Here, we first review clinical studies on sex differences in psychostimulant and opioid craving and relapse. Next, we review preclinical studies on sex differences in psychostimulant and opioid reinstatement of drug seeking after extinction of drug self-administration, and incubation of drug craving (time-dependent increase in drug seeking during abstinence). We also discuss ovarian hormones’ role in relapse and craving in humans and animal models and speculate on brain mechanisms underlying their role in cocaine craving and relapse in rodent models. Finally, we discuss imaging studies on brain responses to cocaine cues and stress in men and women.The results of the clinical studies reviewed do not appear to support the notion that women are more vulnerable to psychostimulant and opioid craving and relapse. However, this conclusion is tentative because most of the studies reviewed were correlational, not sufficiently powered, and not a priori designed to detect sex differences. Additionally, imaging studies suggest sex differences in brain responses to cocaine cues and stress. The results of the preclinical studies reviewed provide evidence for sex differences in stress-induced reinstatement and incubation of cocaine craving but not cue- or cocaine-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking. These sex differences are modulated in part by ovarian hormones. In contrast, the available data do not support the notion of sex differences in craving and relapse/reinstatement for methamphetamine or opioids in rodent models.
Significance Statement This systematic review summarizes clinical and preclinical studies on sex differences in psychostimulant and opioid craving and relapse. Results of the clinical studies reviewed do not appear to support the notion that women are more vulnerable to psychostimulant and opioid craving and relapse. Results of preclinical studies reviewed provide evidence for sex differences in reinstatement and incubation of cocaine seeking but not for reinstatement or incubation of methamphetamine or opioid seeking.
I. Introduction
Substance use disorders are characterized by high relapse rates during abstinence (Hunt et al., 1971; Sinha, 2011; SAMHSA, 2016; Marsh et al., 2018). Over the last decades, investigators examined sex differences in human drug use and relapse (Griffin et al., 1989). In the early 1990s, Kosten et al. (1993) reported that women have more severe cocaine use problems, more cocaine use days, and shorter abstinence periods. In contrast, women had better outcomes at 6-month follow-up (Kosten et al., 1993). In the early 2000s, Elman et al. (2002) reported that cue-induced cocaine craving is higher in women. In parallel, many preclinical studies since the 1990s reported that female rats are more sensitive than males to the reinforcing effects of cocaine, as assessed by drug self-administration and conditioned place preference models (Lynch et al., 2002; Roth and Carroll, 2004; Lynch, 2006; Becker and Hu, 2008; Quinones-Jenab and Jenab, 2012). For example, female rats acquire cocaine self-administration faster than male rats (Lynch and Carroll, 1999). Additionally, investigators reported sex differences in relapse to cocaine seeking, as assessed by extinction-reinstatement and incubation of drug craving models (Becker, 2016; Carroll and Lynch, 2016; Becker et al., 2017) (see below). These findings have led to the widely held dogma especially in the preclinical addiction field that across drug classes females are more vulnerable to initiation and escalation of drug use and to relapse to drug use during abstinence (Fattore et al., 2008; Becker, 2016; Carroll and Lynch, 2016; Becker and Chartoff, 2019).
The goal of our review is to critically examine evidence for sex differences in psychostimulant and opioid craving and relapse in humans and in reinstatement of drug seeking and incubation of drug craving in rat models. We refer readers to excellent comprehensive reviews of the preclinical literature on sex differences in initiation and escalation of drug self-administration and withdrawal (Lynch et al., 2002; Roth et al., 2004; Becker and Koob, 2016; Becker and Chartoff, 2019). Notably, our review does not cover sex differences in humans and animal models of legal drugs (nicotine and alcohol). We refer readers to excellent papers and reviews on this topic (Walitzer and Dearing, 2006; Rahman et al., 2016; Goenaga et al., 2020).
We first review clinical studies on sex differences in psychostimulant (cocaine, methamphetamine) and opioid (heroin) craving and relapse. We used the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines for the literature search strategy and searched Embase, PsycINFO, and MEDLINE databases. The details of the search strategy for MEDLINE are provided below, which was adapted for the other two databases using similar terms (see methods of the systematic review in the Supplemental Material for more details):
(“craving”[MeSH] OR “craving”[tiab] OR “recurrence”[MeSH] OR “relapse”[tiab] OR “abstinence”[tiab] OR “remission”[tiab]) AND (“analgesics opioid”[Pharmacological Action] OR “Central Nervous System Stimulants”[Pharmacological Action] OR “Cocaine”[MeSH] OR “Amphetamine-Related Disorders”[MeSH] OR “Opioid-Related Disorders”[MeSH] OR “Cocaine-Related Disorders”[MeSH] OR “opioid”[tiab] OR “opiate”[tiab] OR “opium”[tiab] OR “heroin”[tiab] OR “morphine”[tiab] OR “stimulants”[tiab] OR “amphetamine”[tiab] OR “dextroamphetamine”[tiab] OR “methamphetamine”[tiab] OR “Cocaine”[tiab] OR “caffeine”[tiab]) AND (“sex characteristics”[MeSH] OR “sex factors”[MeSH] OR “sex characteristics”[tiab] OR “sex differences”[tiab] OR “gender”[tiab] OR “gender differences”[tiab]). Filters: Humans
Next, we review preclinical studies on sex differences in psychostimulant and opioid (heroin, fentanyl, oxycodone) reinstatement of drug seeking and incubation of drug craving. We also discuss the role of ovarian hormones in cocaine craving and relapse/reinstatement in humans and animal models. As with the clinical search, our preclinical literature search followed PRISMA guidelines, and we searched Embase, PsycINFO, and MEDLINE datasets using keywords and controlled vocabulary terms relevant to our review. Below we provide the details of the research strategy for MEDLINE, which was adapted for the other two databases:
((craving[Title/Abstract] OR relapse[Title/Abstract] OR recurrence[Title/Abstract] OR abstinence[Title/Abstract] OR recovery[Title/Abstract] OR remission[Title/Abstract]) AND (“sex differences”[Title/Abstract] OR “sex difference”[Title/Abstract] OR “gender differences”[Title/Abstract] OR “gender difference”[Title/Abstract])) AND (opioid[Title/Abstract] OR methamphetamine[Title/Abstract] OR estrous cycle[Title/Abstract] OR estradiol[Title/Abstract] OR progesterone[Title/Abstract]). Filters: Other Animals
Next, we propose a mechanistic model of the role of ovarian hormones in mediating sex differences in cocaine relapse in rat models. Finally, we summarize results from several human imaging studies on the brain response to drug cues and stress in men and women. In Supplemental Tables S1–S5 and Figs. 1 and 2, we provide a summary of the studies reviewed. In Table 1, we provide a glossary of terms (italic font in the text), and in Table 2, we outline the methodological considerations in studying ovarian hormones in human and rodent models of drug craving and relapse. In the Supplemental Material, we describe the methodological details of the systematic review of both the clinical (Supplemental Fig. 1) and preclinical (Supplemental Fig. 2) literature [note: Our review does not include studies on sex differences in extinction responding 1 day after the last self-administration session because this procedure does not meet the operational definition of relapse (resumption of drug-seeking behavior after a period of abstinence)].
Summary of clinical studies on sex differences in psychostimulant and opioid craving and relapse. Both craving (left) and relapse (right) panels depict the proportion of observations and list the total number of comparisons of sex differences in which F > M, F = M, and F < M for cocaine (A and D), methamphetamine (B and E), and opioids (C and F). Conditions under which craving was measured, and the abstinence period when relapse was assessed is also displayed. F, females; M, males; m, months. Note: The number of comparisons does not equal the number of studies for a given category in Supplemental Table S1 because in some studies investigators assessed more than one dependent measure (e.g., both cue- and stress-induced craving). Spontaneous craving refers to baseline nonprovoked subjective craving.
Glossary of terms
Methodological considerations in studying the role of ovarian hormones in drug craving and relapse in humans and animal models
II. Sex Differences in Drug Craving and Relapse: Clinical Studies
We review studies on sex differences in psychostimulant and opioid craving and relapse in human laboratory and treatment settings (Fig. 1; Supplemental Table S1). Drug craving is correlated, to some degree, with drug use (Bordnick and Schmitz, 1998; Da Silveira et al., 2006; Paliwal et al., 2008; Preston et al., 2009, 2018) and often predictive of future relapse (Bordnick and Schmitz, 1998; Sinha et al., 2006, 2011). In human laboratory studies, drug craving is measured by psychometric self-report assessments before and after exposure to the drug itself, drug-related cues, or stressors (Jaffe et al., 1989; Sinha et al., 2011). Cue-induced craving is achieved by exposing study participants to videos or pictures showing drug-associated cues (e.g., syringe), handling of drug paraphernalia, standardized scripts, or individualized guided imagery (Bedi et al., 2011). Stress-induced craving is achieved by exposing study participants to psychologic (e.g., personalized imagery, public speaking, mental arithmetic), physical (e.g., cold pressor), or pharmacological (e.g., α-2 adrenoreceptor antagonist yohimbine) stressors (Charney et al., 1983; Sinha et al., 1999, 2011; Stine et al., 2002).
In treatment settings, relapse is defined as resumption of regular drug use during outpatient treatment or after completion of inpatient or outpatient treatment. These studies rely on follow-up interviews at specific time points or ecological momentary assessment, real-time reporting of drug craving, and use in the natural environment (Shiffman et al., 1996; Epstein et al., 2009). We also review cross-sectional studies because they provide additional insight on sex differences in relapse vulnerability.
A. Psychostimulants
1. Craving
Several studies examined sex differences in cocaine craving (Fig. 1; Supplemental Table S1), and although some studies suggest greater craving in women, other studies do not.
2. Spontaneous Drug Craving
Cocaine (Fig. 1A): Elman et al. (2001) reported that when craving is assessed during early abstinence (12 hours) with a craving questionnaire of six items measuring aspects of craving, such as current intensity, projected intensity, resistance, and response to drug-associated cue, women reported higher spontaneous (nonprovoked) cocaine craving than men. However, other studies found no sex differences in cocaine craving 2 days into abstinence (Waldrop et al., 2010) or in nonabstaining users (Volkow et al., 2011) when craving was measured with the Craving/Distress/Mood scale or a brief version of the Cocaine Craving Questionnaire. Similarly, when craving is monitored at least a week into abstinence with a brief version of the Cocaine Craving Questionnaire or a 10-point visual analog scale, several studies reported no sex differences in spontaneous craving (Fox et al., 2006, 2013; Paliwal et al., 2008).
Methamphetamine (Fig. 1B): Galloway et al. (2010) reported no sex differences in spontaneous methamphetamine craving measured by a self-reported 0–100 scale (endpoint anchors of “no craving” and “most craving ever experienced”) during 4 months of abstinence.
3. Drug-Induced Craving
Cocaine (Fig. 1A): During early abstinence (<7 days), craving induced by cocaine injection was either similar in men and women when measured by multidimensional questionnaires (Elman et al., 2002) or higher in men when measured with 10-point visual analog scales (Evans et al., 1999).
4. Cue-Induced Craving
Cocaine (Fig. 1A): No sex differences in cue-induced cocaine craving were observed after 2 days of abstinence (Waldrop et al., 2010) or in nonabstaining users (Volkow et al., 2011). Similarly, Fox et al. (2006) and Potenza et al. (2012) reported no sex differences in cue-induced craving when craving was measured at least a week into abstinence using a 10-point visual analog scale. Additionally, using the same measurement tool, there were no sex differences in cocaine craving in individuals with comorbid substance use disorder undergoing long-term opioid agonist treatment (Avants et al., 1995; Kennedy et al., 2013). However, Fox et al. (2014) and Robbins et al. (1999) reported higher cue-induced cocaine craving measured with a 10-point visual analog scale in women after 14–21 days of inpatient treatment or long-term outpatient treatment, respectively.
Methamphetamine (Fig. 1B): Tolliver et al. (2010) reported no sex differences in cue-induced methamphetamine craving measured with a 10-point visual analog scale during early abstinence.
5. Stress-Induced Craving
Cocaine (Fig. 1A): Waldrop et al. (2010) reported stronger correlation between peak craving and peak response to a social stressor (Trier Social Stress Test) in women after 2 days of abstinence. Similarly, Moran-Santa Maria et al. (2014) reported that within 2–3 days of initiating abstinence, women report greater yohimbine-induced craving measured with a 10-point visual analog scale. In contrast, using the same craving scale method, Back et al. (2005) reported no sex differences in craving induced by psychological (Mental Arithmetic Task) or physical (Cold Pressor Task) stressors. Additionally, Brady et al. (2009) reported that after similar abstinence length, men and women show similar corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF)-induced craving. Finally, no sex differences in craving induced by script-guided stressful situations were observed in participants abstinent for at least 2 weeks using a 10-point visual analog scale (Li et al., 2005).
Together, these studies show that women are more sensitive to stress-induced craving only during early abstinence and that this effect is independent of the type of stressor (e.g., physical or psychosocial). However, our conclusion is tentative because of the few studies published, the divergent results, and the low statistical power to measure sex differences.
6. Relapse
Cocaine (Fig. 1D): Kosten et al. (1993) reported that upon entering treatment, women had higher relapse rates. However, they reported that there were no sex differences during the treatment study (desipramine or lithium carbonate), and at 6-month follow-up, relapse rates were lower in women (Kosten et al., 1993). In contrast, Kennedy et al. (2013) reported that in polydrug (cocaine + heroin) users, women undergoing opioid agonist therapy showed higher relapse rates over a 7-month period. However, during 3-month (Burch et al., 2015; Bashiri et al., 2017) and 6-month (Gallop et al., 2007) treatment follow-up, women showed lower relapse rates. Similarly, when relapse was assessed at treatment follow-up, women often exhibited better outcomes. Specifically, women had lower relapse rates at 6-month follow-up from both inpatient (Weiss et al., 1997) and outpatient (Kosten et al., 1993) treatment. In contrast, no sex differences in relapse rates were observed 9 months (Sterling et al., 2004) or 12 months (Negrete and Emil, 1992; McKay et al., 2003) after outpatient treatment. Additionally, during a 2-year period after a randomized clinical trial in polydrug (cocaine + opioid) users, men and women showed similar relapse rates (Schottenfeld et al., 1998).
Methamphetamine (Fig. 1E): Hillhouse et al. (2007) reported higher relapse rates in women during 4-month outpatient treatment but no sex differences in relapse at 6-month or 12-month post-treatment. In contrast, Lanyon et al. (2019) reported higher relapse rates in men at both 12-month and 5-year follow-ups. He et al. (2013) and Brecht et al. (2000, 2004) reported no sex differences in lifetime relapse rates among inpatients or after treatment completion.
B. Opioids
1. Craving
Back et al. (2011) reported greater spontaneous opioid craving in women, and Yu et al. (2007) reported higher cue-induced heroin craving in women during inpatient treatment (Fig. 1C). Additionally, Moran et al. (2018) reported that over 4 months of outpatient opioid maintenance therapy, women showed higher stress-induced opioid craving. However, Herbeck et al. (2016) reported no sex differences in opioid craving during 3 weeks of extended-release naltrexone treatment. Similarly, Kennedy et al. (2013) reported no sex differences in heroin craving during 6 months of outpatient opioid agonist treatment with methadone (100 mg/day).
2. Relapse
Maehira et al. (2013) reported higher relapse rates in women during the first 2 months of abstinence (Fig. 1F). Ignjatova and Raleva (2009) reported more heroin lapses in women during 6 months of opioid agonist treatment with methadone (dose not provided). In contrast, Kamal et al. (2007) and Kennedy et al. (2013) reported no sex differences in heroin relapse during the first 3 (methadone, average 74 mg/day) and 6 (methadone, 100 mg/day) months of opioid agonist treatment.
After more prolonged abstinence periods (>1 year), there is some evidence for lower relapse rates in women. Gordon et al. (2017) reported lower relapse rates for women at 1-year follow-up after buprenorphine treatment (average 8 mg/day). Zimmer-Höfler and Dobler-Mikola (1992) reported similar findings after 2 years of abstinence from different therapeutic programs (e.g., methadone maintenance therapy, therapeutic community program) and prisons. Additionally, during a 2-year follow-up period during opioid agonist treatment (buprenorphine, 4 mg/day) for polydrug (cocaine + opioids) use, women had more drug-free days (Schottenfeld et al., 1998). Within the same longitudinal study of individuals using heroin who were recruited from randomly selected treatment agencies delivering various treatments (e.g., methadone or buprenorphine maintenance treatment, drug-free residential rehabilitation, or detoxification), Darke et al. reported that women were less likely to relapse over 3-year (Darke et al., 2007) and 11-year (Darke et al., 2015) follow-up periods. These results contrast with previous studies reporting no sex differences in relapse at 7-year (Zimmer-Höfler and Dobler-Mikola, 1992) and 8-year (Brunswick and Messeri, 1986) follow-ups. Additionally, several studies reported no sex differences in relapse over 12-month follow-up during methadone treatment [average dose 130 mg/day in Adelson et al. (2018) and 40–70 mg/day in Levine et al. (2015) and Smyth et al. (2012)] and no sex differences in lifetime opioid relapse rates (Zhou et al., 2017; Moradinazar et al., 2020). Importantly, the different outcomes from these studies could be due to the divergent screening of the participants. Indeed, although some studies directly compared drug craving and relapse in men and women after the same opioid agonist treatment (e.g., methadone or buprenorphine maintenance therapy), others compared cohorts of participants with mixed therapy programs (e.g., opioid agonist therapy, detoxification, or drug-free residential rehabilitation), which may increase sample variability and decrease the power to detect sex differences.
C. Conclusions
The studies reviewed do not support the notion of sex differences in drug craving and relapse for either psychostimulants or opioids. However, these studies suggest areas for further examination, including potential sex differences in craving and relapse vulnerability during early abstinence wherein women may be more vulnerable.
III. Sex Differences in Drug Craving and Relapse: Preclinical Studies
Sex differences in drug relapse were examined using the reinstatement and incubation of drug-craving models (Fig. 2; Supplemental Table S2) (Venniro et al., 2016). In studies using the reinstatement model, investigators determined sex differences in resumption of drug seeking after extinction induced by exposure to the self-administered drug, discrete cues, contextual cues, and stress. For clarity, we refer to discrete cues as “cues” in the text below. In stress-induced reinstatement studies, the typical stressors investigators used in male rats were intermittent foot shock (Shaham and Stewart, 1995; Shaham et al., 2000; Mantsch et al., 2016) and yohimbine as a pharmacological stressor (Shepard et al., 2004; Mantsch et al., 2016). In opioid users, yohimbine has been shown to induce stress- and withdrawal-like symptoms and opioid craving (Stine et al., 2002). However, interpretation of results from studies using yohimbine in reference to stress-induced reinstatement has been challenged by Chen et al. (2015). They showed that at the dose range yohimbine is used in reinstatement studies, the drug’s effect on reinstatement is independent of the history of contingent self-administration and unrelated to the commonly assumed stress-like effects of yohimbine.
Summary of preclinical studies on sex differences in psychostimulant and opioid reinstatement and incubation of craving. Both reinstatement of drug-seeking behavior (left) and incubation of craving and drug seeking after forced abstinence (right) panels depict the proportion of observations and list the total number of comparisons of sex differences in which F > M, F = M, and F < M for cocaine (A and D), methamphetamine (B and E), and opioids (C and F). The conditions under which drug seeking was measured are also displayed. F, females; M, males; Yoh, yohimbine. Note: the number of comparisons does not equal the number of studies for a given category in Supplemental Table S2 because in some studies investigators assessed more than one dependent measure (e.g., both cue- and stress-induced reinstatement).
A. Psychostimulants
1. Drug-Induced Reinstatement
Cocaine (Fig. 2A): Lynch and Carroll (2000) reported that female rats showed higher drug-induced reinstatement (often termed drug priming-induced reinstatement) after short-access drug self-administration of cocaine at 1.0 and 3.2 mg/kg but not 0.32 mg/kg. This initial finding of sex differences in cocaine-induced reinstatement was confirmed in subsequent studies (Anker et al., 2009; Smith et al., 2012; Doncheck et al., 2020). In contrast, after continuous long-access drug self-administration of cocaine, Zlebnik et al. (2014) and Swalve et al. (2016) reported no sex differences in cocaine-induced reinstatement. Additionally, after intermittent-access drug self-administration of cocaine, Kawa and Robinson (2019) reported no sex differences in reinstatement induced by cocaine priming (<1.6 mg/kg, i.v.).
A tentative conclusion from these studies is that females are more sensitive to cocaine-induced reinstatement after short-access but not long-access cocaine self-administration, suggesting that cocaine history (e.g., quantity of drug intake) may be an important contributing factor to sex differences in cocaine-induced reinstatement.
Finally, Jordan and Andersen (2018) reported that after 30 days of short-access self-administration training with a high (0.75 mg/kg) but not low (0.25 mg/kg) cocaine unit dose (training started on P28) followed by 30 abstinence days, cocaine seeking was higher in males than in females after exposure to a noncontingent cocaine injection (10 mg/kg, i.p) immediately prior to a single relapse test session under extinction conditions. The relevance of these results to sex differences in cocaine-induced reinstatement after extinction is unknown because the rats did not undergo extinction training and the authors did not assess the effect of vehicle (saline) injections on drug seeking.
Methamphetamine (Fig. 2B): After both short- and long-access drug self-administration, methamphetamine (1 mg/kg)-induced reinstatement was higher in female rats (Holtz et al., 2012; Reichel et al., 2012; Cox et al., 2013; Cordie and McFadden, 2019). However, using the same dose, others reported either no sex differences in methamphetamine-induced reinstatement (Everett et al., 2020; Zlebnik et al., 2021) or higher reinstatement in males (Everett et al., 2021).
2. Cue-Induced Reinstatement
Cocaine (Fig. 2A): Zhou et al. (2014) reported higher cue-induced reinstatement in females after short-access cocaine self-administration. In contrast, under similar training conditions, several studies reported no sex differences in cue-induced reinstatement (Fuchs et al., 2005; Swalve et al., 2016; Bechard et al., 2018; Weber et al., 2018). Similarly, no sex differences in cue-induced reinstatement were observed after long-access (Zlebnik et al., 2014) or intermittent-access (Kawa and Robinson, 2019) cocaine self-administration.
Methamphetamine (Fig. 2B): Five studies examined sex differences in cue-induced reinstatement of methamphetamine seeking, and, as with cocaine, the results are inconclusive. After short-access self-administration, Cox et al. (2013) reported higher cue-induced reinstatement in female rats. In contrast, Bernheim et al. (2017) and Everett et al. (2020a) reported no sex differences. Additionally, Takashima et al., (2018) reported higher cue-induced reinstatement in male rats after long-access self-administration. Zlebnik et al. (2021) used a methamphetamine self-administration protocol of two 2-hour sessions/day and reported no sex differences in cue-induced reinstatement.
3. Stress-Induced Reinstatement
Cocaine (Fig. 2A): After short-access self-administration, Anker and Carroll (2010b) reported higher yohimbine-induced reinstatement, and Feltenstein et al. (2011) reported higher yohimbine- and cue-induced reinstatement in females. In contrast, Zlebnik et al. (2014) reported no sex differences in either yohimbine-induced or yohimbine- and cue-induced reinstatement. Doncheck et al. (2020) reported no sex differences in restraint stress-induced potentiation of drug-induced reinstatement. However, they reported that intermittent footshock potentiates drug-induced reinstatement in males but not females. In contrast, Connelly et al. (2020) reported higher foot shock–induced reinstatement in females.
In male rats, the effect of intermittent footshock stress on reinstatement of drug seeking is inhibited by extrahypothalamic CRF, and ventricular CRF injections mimic the effect of intermittent footshock on reinstatement (Shaham et al., 2000; Erb et al., 2001; Mantsch et al., 2016). Based on these results, Buffalari et al. (2012) examined sex differences in CRF-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking after short-access self-administration. The main finding was that although ventricular injections of CRF reinstated cocaine seeking in both sexes, the response to CRF was more variable in females than in males.
Methamphetamine (Fig. 2B): Cox et al. (2013) reported higher yohimbine-induced reinstatement in females, whereas Everett et al. (2020a) reported no sex differences. The reasons for these different results are unclear but may be due to a long abstinence period (21 days) before the extinction phase in the Everett et al. (2020a) study. As discussed above, interpretation of data from yohimbine studies in reference to stress-induced reinstatement is problematic (Chen et al., 2015; Mantsch et al., 2016).
Together, these divergent results across different stressors do not indicate significant sex differences in stress-induced reinstatement of cocaine or methamphetamine seeking.
4. Incubation of Craving and Relapse after Abstinence
Cocaine (Fig. 2D): Kerstetter et al. (2008) reported longer-lasting and higher incubation of cocaine craving (up to 180 days of home-cage forced abstinence) in females after long-access drug self-administration. We extended these results and reported higher incubation of cocaine craving on abstinence day 29 in females after both long-access and intermittent-access cocaine self-administration (Nicolas et al., 2019) (Fig. 3). Most recently, Corbett et al. (2021) reported similar results (i.e., higher incubation in females) after both 15 and 48 abstinence days. Johnson et al. (2019) reported similar results with higher cocaine seeking in female rats tested at 1 or 30 abstinence days after short-access cocaine self-administration. However, these results should be interpreted with caution because in both sexes the incubation effect was variable and statistically nonsignificant, likely because of the use of the short-access procedure [incubation is less robust with this procedure (Lu et al., 2004)]. Additionally, Madangopal et al. (2019) reported that incubation of the response to cocaine discriminative cues (e.g., light, tone) that predict availability of the drug is more persistent in female rats and lasts for up to 200 abstinence days. However, these results should also be interpreted with caution because the study was not powered to detect sex differences.
Effect of estrous cycle on incubation of craving after long-access and intermittent-access cocaine self-administration in female rats. Relapse (incubation) test. Mean ± S.E.M. number of active lever presses per session after continuous (nonestrus: n = 12 for day 2 and 29, estrus: n = 9/8 for day 2/29) and intermittent (nonestrus: n = 13/16 for day 2/29, estrus n = 10/7 for days 2/29) access drug self-administration. * Different from day 2 within each estrous phase, P < 0.05; # Different from nonestrus on day 29, P < 0.05. Adapted from Nicolas et al. (2019). Figure 3 was reproduced with permission from Elsevier.
Schematic comparison of the menstrual cycle in humans and the estrous cycle in rodents and fluctuation of estradiol and progesterone levels across the cycle phases. In humans, the menstrual cycle is divided into the follicular and luteal phases separated by ovulation (Sherman and Korenman, 1975; Anker and Carroll, 2010a). The cycle begins with menses, and the onset of the follicular phase is characterized by high levels of estradiol, with a peak during the preovulation period before decreasing to a moderate level during the luteal phase. Conversely, progesterone is at its lowest level during the follicular phase and starts increasing during the preovulation period to peak at the midluteal phase. In female rodents, the estrous cycle is divided into late diestrus, proestrus, estrus, and early diestrus phases, with ovulation occurring between proestrus and estrus (Cora et al., 2015; Krentzel and Meitzen, 2018). Estradiol peaks twice at the middle of the early diestrus and proestrus phases and drops by ovulation. Progesterone peaks at the end of the early diestrus and proestrus phases, and low stable levels remain for the rest of the cycle. In many studies, investigators pool together early and late diestrus phases to a single phase called diestrus because of similar hormonal and cytologic characteristics. Additionally, when no behavioral differences are observed during proestrus and diestrus, investigators combine these phases and call the combined phase nonestrus. There are major differences in the reproductive cycle of women and female rodents: the duration of the cycle, the cycle pattern of estradiol and progesterone, and the amplitude of hormone level variations. However, when the hormonal ratio over the phases is compared, some analogies are conventionally made: the follicular phase is comparable to the estrus phase (progesterone < estradiol) and the luteal phase to the nonestrus phase (progesterone > estradiol) (Anker and Carroll, 2010a; Krentzel and Meitzen, 2018).
In contrast, Venniro et al. (2021) reported that after either long-access (12 hour/day) or intermittent long-access (12 hour/day, 5 minutes ON, 25 minutes OFF ×24), there were no sex differences in incubation of cocaine craving after home-cage forced abstinence or after voluntary abstinence induced by providing rats with a mutually exclusive choice between cocaine or rewarding social interaction. The different results of Venniro et al. (2021) versus the studies of Kerstetter et al. (2008), Nicolas et al. (2019), and Corbett et al. (2021) may be because of two reasons. The first is that Venniro et al. (2021) trained their rats for 12 hours/day, whereas in the other studies, the session duration was 6 hours/day. The second is that the male and female rats in Venniro et al. (2021) study were first trained to lever press for social interaction for 6 days, whereas the rats in the other studies were not.
Methamphetamine (Fig. 2D): Venniro et al. (2017) (long-access self-administration) and Everett et al. (2020a) (short- and long-access self-administration) reported no sex differences in incubation of methamphetamine craving after either forced abstinence in home cage or voluntary abstinence in the drug self-administration chambers, the latter being achieved by providing rats with mutually exclusive choices between the self-administered drug and an alternative nondrug reinforcer (e.g., palatable food or social interaction) (Caprioli et al., 2015; Caprioli et al., 2017; Venniro et al., 2018; Venniro and Shaham, 2020; Fredriksson et al., 2021). Similarly, Daiwile et al. (2019) reported no sex differences in incubation of methamphetamine craving after forced abstinence after a self-administration protocol of two 3-hour sessions/day. In contrast, Ruda-Kucerova et al. (2015, 2017) reported that in a single relapse test after 15 days of forced abstinence from short-access methamphetamine self-administration, drug seeking was higher in female rats. In the context of sex differences in incubation of drug craving, these results are difficult to interpret because the authors did not establish that incubation had occurred under their experimental conditions.
B. Opioids
1. Drug-Induced Reinstatement (Fig. 2C)
In an early study, Klein et al. (1997) reported that reacquisition of oral fentanyl self-administration after extinction is higher in female rats. However, these results are difficult to interpret without ruling out potential confounds related to the oral route of drug administration (i.e., sex differences in pharmacokinetics and taste sensitivity). In a recent study, Malone et al. (2021) tested both sexes for fentanyl-induced reinstatement after short- (1 hour) or long- (6 hours) access fentanyl self-administration training. They reported higher fentanyl-induced reinstatement in females after short- but not long-access fentanyl self-administration. Similarly, Smethells et al. (2020) reported higher heroin-induced reinstatement and heroin- and cue-induced reinstatement after short-access self-administration.
2. Cue- and Context-Induced Reinstatement (Fig. 2C)
Cue-induced reinstatement: Vazquez et al. (2019) reported higher extinction responding and cue-induced reinstatement of heroin seeking in females after short-access drug self-administration in food-restricted rats. In contrast, Phillips et al. (2020) reported no sex differences in cue-induced reinstatement of oxycodone self-administration after 3 hours oral oxycodone self-administration in mice. Additionally, Smethells et al. (2020) reported no differences in cue-induced reinstatement after short-access heroin self-administration. Bakhti-Suroosh et al. (2021) tested both sexes for cue-induced reinstatement after 6 hours of extinction in a single daily session performed 14 days after intermittent-access fentanyl self-administration (24 hours/day) under two training conditions: fixed ratio 1 reinforcement schedule with or without 1-second timeout. There were no sex differences in either extinction responding or cue-induced reinstatement in the no-timeout training condition. In contrast, sex differences (higher responding in females) emerged in the 1-second timeout training condition for extinction responding but not cue-induced reinstatement. The reasons for the sex-specific effect of the timeout manipulation on extinction responding are unknown. Most recently, Malone et al. (2021) tested both sexes for cue-induced reinstatement after short- (1 hour) or long- (6 hours) access fentanyl self-administration training. They reported lower cue-induced reinstatement in females after long- but not short-access self-administration.
Context-induced reinstatement: Bossert et al. (2020, 2021) reported no sex differences in context-induced reinstatement of oxycodone or heroin seeking after long-access (6 hours/day) drug self-administration training.
Together, there is limited evidence for sex differences in cue- or context-induced reinstatement of opioid seeking. Unexpectedly, under certain experimental conditions, female rats can be either more sensitive [intermittent access (24 hour/day) plus 1-second timeout] or less sensitive [long-access (6 hours/day) drug self-administration] to cue-induced reinstatement of fentanyl seeking.
3. Stress-Induced Reinstatement (Fig. 2C)
Smethells et al. (2020) reported no sex differences in extinction responding but higher yohimbine-induced reinstatement in females; they also reported higher yohimbine- and cue-induced reinstatement in females after short-access heroin self-administration. In contrast, Fulenwider et al. (2020) reported no sex differences in footshock–induced reinstatement of oxycodone seeking after short-access oral oxycodone self-administration training. Malone et al. (2021) tested male and female rats for yohimbine-induced reinstatement after short- (1 hour) or long- (6 hours) access fentanyl self-administration training. There were no sex differences in either extinction responding or yohimbine-induced reinstatement; however, independent of the access condition, yohimbine’s effect on reinstatement was weak and statistically significant only after injections of the low (1 mg/kg) but not higher (2 mg/kg) dose. In the context of sex differences in stress-induced reinstatement, these data are difficult to interpret because of the weak effect of yohimbine on reinstatement in both sexes. In a follow-up study, the same group studied the effect of systemic injections of the glucocorticoid receptor antagonist PT150 on stress-induced reinstatement of fentanyl seeking (Hammerslag et al., 2021). They reported that PT150 decreased intermittent footshock– but not yohimbine-induced reinstatement in male but not female rats. However, these results should also be interpreted with caution because they are 1) based on post hoc analyses in the absence of a significant interaction between the footshock stress condition and PT150 dose, and 2) the weak effect of yohimbine on reinstatement in both sexes and the weak effect of footshock on reinstatement in females. Finally, the data on the inhibitory effect of glucocorticoid antagonism on stress-induced reinstatement of opioid seeking is unexpected based on previous endocrinological and pharmacological studies showing that the effect of intermittent footshock on reinstatement of drug seeking is independent of activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis and glucocorticoid systems (Shaham et al., 2000; Erb et al., 2001; Mantsch et al., 2016).
Together, the limited number of studies reviewed above do not support the notion of increased vulnerability to stress-induced reinstatement of opioid seeking in females.
4. Incubation of Craving and Relapse during Abstinence (Fig. 2F)
Unlike the mixed evidence for reinstatement of opioid seeking, there is no evidence for sex differences in incubation of opioid craving after either forced or voluntary abstinence achieved via either providing the rats with alternative non–drug reinforcer in a choice procedure or by introducing an electric barrier of increasing shock intensity near the drug-paired lever (Cooper et al., 2007).
Using a long-access self-administration protocol, Venniro et al. (2017) reported no sex differences in either incubation of heroin seeking after forced abstinence or reversal of incubation of heroin craving after food choice–induced abstinence. Venniro et al. (2019) replicated the findings for forced abstinence and also reported no sex differences in the inhibition of incubation of heroin seeking after social choice–induced abstinence. Reiner et al. (2020) reported no sex differences in relapse to fentanyl seeking after 2 weeks of food choice–induced abstinence. Fredriksson et al. (2020) reported no sex differences in incubation of oxycodone seeking after forced abstinence or potentiation of incubation of oxycodone seeking after electric barrier–induced abstinence. Finally, Bossert et al. (2021) reported no sex differences in incubation of heroin seeking after home-cage forced abstinence. In this study, the authors performed the incubation-related tests after 1 and 8 abstinence days (within-subjects assessment) in a nondrug context because the same rats were also tested for context-induced reinstatement after extinction of the operant response in the nondrug context.
C. Conclusions
The studies reviewed demonstrate sex differences for stress-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking and incubation of cocaine craving after forced abstinence. In contrast, there is mixed evidence for sex differences in cue- or drug-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking. In the case of cocaine-induced reinstatement, it appears that females are more vulnerable after short-access but not long-access cocaine self-administration. The literature on sex differences in cocaine pharmacokinetics is mixed, but sex differences in cocaine-induced reinstatement could be at least in part due to sex differences in first-pass cocaine metabolism after intraperitoneal noncontingent cocaine injections (Bowman et al., 1999; Festa et al., 2004). Similarly, the evidence supporting sex differences in reinstatement of methamphetamine and opioid seeking across different reinstating stimuli is mixed at best. Additionally, there is consistent evidence for lack of sex differences in incubation of methamphetamine and opioid craving after forced or voluntary abstinence.
However, these conclusions should be confirmed in future studies considering the relatively small number of sex differences studies, particularly for stress-induced reinstatement, wherein investigators primarily used yohimbine as the stress manipulation. In this regard, the subjects in the first intermittent footshock stress–induced reinstatement study were mostly female rats (Shaham and Stewart, 1995), but with few exceptions (Buffalari et al., 2012; Sedki et al., 2015; Connelly et al., 2020; Doncheck et al., 2020), only males were used in subsequent studies on reinstatement induced by intermittent footshock or other stressors like restraint, food restriction, or forced swim (Mantsch et al., 2016; Reiner et al., 2019). In addition, the studies on sex differences in opioid relapse and craving did not assess opioid dependence by parametric measures of spontaneous or naloxone-precipitated withdrawal symptoms. Consequently, we used the duration of opioid self-administration training as a proxy for nondependent (short-access, 1- to 3-hour daily session) and dependent (long-access, 6 hours or longer daily session) conditions. In this regard, session duration has been shown to be a critical factor in determining the magnitude of relapse to opioid seeking (Ahmed et al., 2000; Reiner et al., 2019).
Finally, studies on reinstatement of food seeking also provide little evidence for sex differences. No sex differences were observed for sucrose-, yohimbine-, or cue-induced reinstatement (Cox et al., 2013; Bernheim et al., 2017; Hernandez et al., 2020) after palatable food self-administration. However, Cox et al. (2013) reported higher cue-induced reinstatement of sucrose seeking in female rats.
IV. Role of Menstrual and Estrous Cycle
Several studies examined the role of ovarian hormones and menstrual/estrous cycle (Figs. 3 and 4) in drug (primarily cocaine) craving and relapse in humans and rat models (Supplemental Tables S3–S4). The methodological aspects and limitations of menstrual and estrous cycle tracking are summarized in Table 2.
A. Clinical Studies
Sofuoglu et al. (1999) reported lower cocaine desire (craving) during the luteal than the follicular phase. Evans et al. (2002) reported lower cocaine-induced subjective positive drug effects and craving during the luteal than the follicular phase. These results suggest a protective effect of progesterone (high during luteal phase) on cocaine-induced cocaine craving. In agreement with this idea, several studies reported that women with high endogenous progesterone levels (comparable to luteal phase) are less sensitive to stress- and cue-induced cocaine craving than women with low progesterone levels (Sinha et al., 2007; Moran-Santa Maria et al., 2018; Sherman et al., 2020). Additionally, several studies reported that exogenous progesterone reduces positive subjective cocaine effects and craving in both women and men (Sofuoglu et al., 2002; Sofuoglu et al., 2004; Evans and Foltin, 2006; Fox et al., 2013; Milivojevic et al., 2016), confirming progesterone’s protective effects.
In contrast, in occasional intranasal cocaine users, Lukas et al. (1996) reported no effect of the menstrual cycle on cocaine-induced subjective positive drug effects. The results of this negative study agree with those from other studies in cocaine-dependent women (Collins et al., 2007; Potenza et al., 2012). Additionally, Fox et al. (2008) reported no variations in craving over the menstrual cycle during the first month of abstinence from smoked cocaine, but the ratio of estradiol/progesterone was stable across the cycle, which could explain these negative results.
However, both the negative and positive results reviewed above should be interpreted with caution because of relatively small sample sizes and other factors that may play a role in the behavioral outcomes under investigation (e.g., severity of cocaine use disorder, route of administration).
B. Preclinical Studies
The results of studies on the role of estrous cycle and ovarian hormones in reinstatement and incubation of craving are summarized in Supplemental Table S4.
Cocaine: Cocaine-induced reinstatement is higher during estrus than diestrus or proestrus (Kippin et al., 2005 Feltenstein and See, 2007; Kerstetter et al., 2008; Feltenstein et al., 2009). The expression of incubation of cocaine craving is higher during estrus than nonestrus (Fig. 3) (Kerstetter et al., 2008; Nicolas et al., 2019; Corbett et al., 2021). In contrast, evidence for the role of estrous cycle in cue- and stress-induced reinstatement is mixed. Fuchs et al. (2005) reported lower cue-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking in females in estrus than nonestrus. Feltenstein et al. (2011) reported that yohimbine- and cue-induced reinstatement is lower in estrus and diestrus than proestrus. In contrast, they reported that the estrus phase has no effect on yohimbine-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking. Peterson et al. (2014) and Bechard et al. (2018) also reported similar cue-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking during different phases of the estrous cycle.
Suppression of ovarian hormones by ovariectomy decreases cocaine-induced reinstatement, whereas chronic estradiol treatment in ovariectomized rats restores this reinstatement to levels similar to those of sham rats (Larson et al., 2005; Anker et al., 2007). Additionally, acute proestrus-level estradiol in ovariectomized rats potentiates cocaine-induced reinstatement (Doncheck et al., 2018). In contrast, exogenous progesterone treatment in free-cycling rats decreases cocaine-induced reinstatement (Anker et al., 2007; Feltenstein et al., 2009). Anker et al. (2007) also reported that exogenous progesterone treatment in ovariectomized rats inhibits the facilitating effect of estradiol on cocaine-induced reinstatement. Together, these results suggest a role of ovarian hormones in cocaine relapse with estradiol increasing relapse vulnerability and progesterone having an opposite effect.
Methamphetamine, heroin, and fentanyl: The role of ovarian hormones in relapse/reinstatement to methamphetamine and opioid seeking is largely unknown. Cox et al. (2013) reported that the estrous cycle has no effect on methamphetamine-induced reinstatement. Vazquez et al. (2019) reported that estradiol or progesterone treatment has no effect on cue-induced reinstatement of heroin seeking. Sedki et al. (2015) reported that food restriction for 2 weeks (a chronic stressor) increases relapse to heroin seeking after home-cage forced abstinence in female rats. This effect is not decreased by ovariectomy, and unexpectedly, in ovariectomized rats, estradiol replacement but not progesterone injections decrease the potentiation effect of chronic food restriction on relapse. These results highlight the necessity to better characterize the potential role of the estrous cycle in heroin relapse.
Bakhti-Suroosh et al. (2021) reported higher extinction responding and cue-induced reinstatement of drug seeking after 14 days of forced abstinence from intermittent-access fentanyl self-administration during estrus versus nonestrus. In contrast, Malone et al. (2021) reported that the estrous cycle has no effect on cue-, yohimbine-, and fentanyl-induced reinstatement after short- or long-access fentanyl self-administration training. These different results may be due to the use of the intermittent access self-administration procedure for 23 hours/day and imposing 14 days abstinence before the relapse/reinstatement tests in the study of Bakhti-Suroosh et al. (2021) versus the use of continuous long-access self-administration procedures for 6 hours/day without an abstinence period in the study conducted by Malone et al. (2021).
C. Conclusions
The clinical and preclinical studies reviewed above suggest that under certain conditions, cocaine craving and relapse are dependent on the menstrual/estrous cycle, with higher vulnerability during the follicular/estrus phase. The hypothesis that emerges from the studies reviewed is that progesterone decreases cocaine craving and relapse vulnerability, whereas estradiol has opposite effects. Specifically, the menstrual/estrous cycle influences incubation of cocaine craving and cocaine-induced reinstatement. However, the review of the literature does not provide clear evidence for sex differences in the magnitude of cocaine-induced reinstatement, indicating that the influence of hormonal cycle on relapse-related behaviors in females does not necessarily lead to higher relapse vulnerability in females compared with males.
Many of the studies reviewed were designed to specifically assess the influence of the menstrual/estrous cycle on cocaine relapse and craving with statistical power to detect the effect of hormonal fluctuations and manipulation on relapse-related behaviors if such effect exists. However, some limitations of the methods to identify menstrual and estrous cycles phases need to be taken into consideration in the interpretation of the results. We describe these limitations in Table 2.
Finally, the results of Sedki et al. (2015) described above on the “protective” effect of estradiol suggest that this hypothesis may not generalize to opioid drugs [but see Bakhti-Suroosh et al. (2021) for results congruent with the notion of opposing roles of estradiol and progesterone in drug relapse].
V. Brain Mechanisms
The brain mechanisms involved in the putative opposite effects of estradiol and progesterone on cocaine craving and relapse are unknown. We speculate that the mesolimbic dopamine system, with its projections from ventral tegmental area (VTA) to nucleus accumbens (NAc), is critically involved. This system plays important roles in both reinstatement of cocaine seeking after extinction (Shalev et al., 2002; Kalivas and McFarland, 2003; Schmidt et al., 2005) and incubation of cocaine craving (Wolf, 2016; Dong et al., 2017). Many studies reported that amphetamine-induced striatal and accumbal dopamine release is increased during estrus in free-cycling females (Becker and Ramirez, 1981; Becker and Cha, 1989; Castner et al., 1993) and by estradiol treatment in ovariectomized rats (Becker, 1990; Thompson and Moss, 1994; Becker and Rudick, 1999; Cummings et al., 2014; Song et al., 2019). In contrast, progesterone has an opposite effect by inhibiting striatal dopamine release in estradiol-primed ovariectomized females (Dluzen and Ramirez, 1984; Becker and Rudick, 1999). Calipari et al. (2017) also reported that dopamine activity in the VTA-to-NAc projection is higher during estrus than nonestrus.
We propose that in females estradiol and progesterone exert opposite effects on dopamine neurotransmission in the VTA-to-NAc projection, leading to increased cocaine craving and relapse by estradiol and decreased cocaine craving and relapse by progesterone (Fig. 5). Other neurobiological mechanisms of reinstatement and incubation of cocaine craving, including mesocorticolimbic glutamate transmission (Cornish and Kalivas, 2000; McFarland and Kalivas, 2001; Kalivas et al., 2009; Wolf, 2016; Dong et al., 2017), may also contribute to the sex differences described above. As discussed elsewhere, there are sex differences in brain glutamate systems and drug-induced neuroadaptations in glutamate transmission that may play a role in this regard (Giacometti and Barker, 2020). To date, no evidence has been provided on the role of estradiol and progesterone in relapse-related behavior in males.
A proposed brain mechanism model of the role of estradiol and progesterone in cocaine relapse in rodent models. There is evidence that estradiol potentiates striatal and NAc dopamine release by modulating GABAergic neurotransmission of medium spiny neurons (MSNs) through collaterals synapsing on dopamine neurons (Krentzel and Meitzen, 2018; Yoest et al., 2018). This effect is likely mediated by membrane estrogen receptors (ERs) (membrane-associated ERα and membrane-associated ERβ) expressed in MSNs (Almey et al., 2012). ERα and ERβ antagonists prevent estradiol enhancement of amphetamine-induced dopamine release (Xiao et al., 2003), and overexpression of ERα in the striatum increases the effect of estradiol on K+ induced GABA release (Schultz et al., 2009). Additionally, ERα and ERβ are expressed on VTA dopamine neurons terminals in NAc (Yoest et al., 2018), which play a critical role in reinstatement of cocaine seeking (Shaham et al., 2003; Schmidt et al., 2005). Together, these results suggest a role of ERα and ERβ in estradiol regulation of dopamine neurotransmission and, by implication, in reinstatement of cocaine seeking. Additionally, increased dopamine release by estradiol would be at least in part due to its action on dopamine receptors: striatal dopamine receptor 2 (Drd2) affinity decreases during estrus (Di Paolo et al., 1988), and estradiol injections in ovariectomized rats decrease Drd2 binding in striatum (Bazzett and Becker, 1994). In contrast to estradiol, progesterone decreases striatal dopamine release, as shown in estradiol-primed ovariectomized females treated with progesterone (Dluzen and Ramirez, 1984; Becker and Rudick, 1999). Progesterone and its metabolites are positive allosteric modulators of GABAA receptors (Schumacher et al., 1989; Schumacher and McEwen, 1989; Lambert et al., 1995). Drugs that promote GABAA function (e.g., imidazenil, diazepam) decrease cocaine-induced increases in dopamine release in NAc shell (Giorgetti et al., 1998). Consequently, progesterone could protect against estradiol-induced increases in cocaine seeking by inhibiting NAc dopamine release via increased GABAA receptor transmission. Together, we propose that during the estrus/follicular phase, cocaine- or cocaine cue-induced NAc dopamine release is increased by estradiol through its action on ER in GABAergic medium spiny striatal neurons and dopamine neurons terminals, leading to disinhibition of dopamine neurons and directly enhanced VTA dopamine cell firing via decreased Drd2 signaling, resulting in increased cocaine reinstatement/relapse. In contrast, during nonestrus/luteal phase, high progesterone levels may inhibit dopamine release induced by drug or drug cues through its action on GABAA receptors expressed in VTA dopamine terminals (Brodnik et al., 2019; Lopes et al., 2019), resulting in decreased cocaine relapse. DA, dopamine.
A. Human Imaging Studies on Sex Differences in Response to Cocaine Cues and Stress
During the last several decades, numerous studies have used positron emission tomography (PET) (Volkow et al., 1991) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) (London et al., 1999) to identify brain regions activated (or inhibited) during craving induced by exposure to drug injections (Stein et al., 1998), drug cues (Grant et al., 1996), and stress (Sinha et al., 2005). However, with rare exceptions, these studies were either performed only in male participants or included both sexes but were not designed (lacked power) to assess sex differences. Indeed, in our systematic literature search, we only identified six studies that statistically evaluated sex differences in brain activity during cue- or stress-induced cocaine craving (Supplemental Table S5). We describe these studies below. In the Supplemental Material we also describe results from several studies wherein investigators separately analyzed imaging data within each sex. The data from these studies are inconclusive and difficult to interpret without knowing whether the interactions between sex and cue or stress conditions are significant (see Nieuwenhuis et al., 2011).
In the studies described below, cue-induced craving was provoked by either presentation of videos or pictures showing cocaine-associated cues or by standardized or personalized scripts (Kilts et al., 2004; Volkow et al., 2011; Zhang et al., 2020). Stress-induced craving was induced by standardized or personalized scripts (Sinha et al., 1999; Li et al., 2005). Brain activity changes induced by cocaine or stress cues were compared with those induced by neutral cues. Participants in these studies were either abstinent and underwent inpatient or outpatient treatment (Kilts et al., 2004; Li et al., 2005; Zhang et al., 2020) or were non–treatment-seeking active cocaine users (Volkow et al., 2011).
Kilts et al. (2004) used PET with [15O]H2O to measure regional cerebral blood flow (an index of brain activity) after cue exposure during early abstinence (days 1–14) in eight women. They compared the women to five men from their previous study (Kilts et al., 2001) and three new men. There were no sex differences in cue-induced cocaine craving. Exposure to cocaine cues induced stronger activation (increased glucose utilization) in right amygdala, left insula, right postcentral gyrus, and left caudate nucleus in men. In contrast, women showed increased activation of right precentral gyrus, middle frontal gyrus, and posterior cingulate gyrus. These results should be interpreted with caution because of the low number of participants and the use of male data from a previous study.
Volkow et al. (2011) used PET with 2-deoxy-2[18F]fluoro-d-glucose to measure brain glucose metabolism in response to cocaine cues in 10 women and 16 men. There were no sex differences in cue-induced cocaine craving. In contrast, sex differences were detected in cue-induced whole-brain metabolism (glucose utilization) with significantly decreased activity in women and modest increased activity in men. Additionally, analyses of specific brain areas showed decreased glucose utilization in frontal, cingulate, and parietal cortices, thalamus, and midbrain in women. In contrast, men showed increased glucose utilization in right inferior frontal gyrus. Direct comparisons of men and women showed sex by cue (cocaine cue, neutral cue) interaction due to greater decrease in glucose utilization among women in frontal (broca areas 8, 9, 10), anterior cingulate, posterior cingulate, inferior parietal, and dorsomedial thalamus.
The reasons for the different pattern of results in the two aforementioned studies, cue-induced brain activation in women in Kilts et al. (2004) versus cue-induced inhibition in Volkow et al. (2011), are unknown. One potential reason is that participants in the first study (Kilts et al., 2004) were treatment seekers tested during abstinence, whereas those in the second study (Volkow et al., 2011) were non–treatment-seeking tested during active cocaine use.
Kober et al. (2016) investigated fMRI-based changes in brain activity in response to videos depicting cocaine use, gambling, or sad scenarios in participants with cocaine dependence or pathologic gambling. There were no sex differences in cocaine craving or urges between men (n = 18) and women (n = 12) with cocaine-dependence. However, men showed greater dorsomedial prefrontal cortex and superior frontal gyrus activation in response to the cocaine videos.
Joseph et al. (2019) used fMRI to investigate the effects of oxytocin on cocaine craving and cocaine cue-induced activity in right amygdala and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (PFC) among cocaine-dependent participants with (24 men, 16 women) or without (19 men, 8 women) childhood trauma history. Independent of the trauma condition, there were no sex differences in cue-induced cocaine craving, and in both sexes, oxytocin had no effect on this measure. Independent of the trauma condition, there were no sex differences in the placebo groups for cue-induced activation of dorsomedial PFC, a finding different from that of Kober et al. (2016), and oxytocin decreased this activation in all groups in a sex-independent manner. A different pattern of results was observed for cue-induced activation of right amygdala, wherein sex differences (activation in men but not women) were observed in the trauma but not nontrauma participants. Additionally, in the trauma groups, oxytocin increased cue-induced right amygdala activity in women but decreased this activity in men. In contrast, oxytocin had no effect on cue-induced right amygdala activity in men and women without trauma. The small sample size of women with prior trauma limits the interpretation of the sex-specific effects of oxytocin on amygdala activity in this study.
Zhang et al. (2020) used fMRI to investigate periaqueductal gray (PAG) activity and connectivity between PAG and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) after craving induced by cue exposure during early abstinence (7–10 days) in 10 women and 42 men. The PAG is known for its role in pain, avoidance, and defensive behaviors (Basbaum and Fields, 1984; Graeff, 1994). There were no sex differences in cue-induced cocaine craving, cue-induced PAG activity, or cue-induced increased PAG-vmPFC connectivity. However, PAG-vmPFC connectivity strength was positively correlated with cue-induced craving in men but negatively correlated in women. These results suggest sex differences in the role of PAG-vmPFC connectivity in cue-induced cocaine craving. However, these results should be interpreted with caution because of the low number of women participants and other significant sex differences in the sample (age and depression score).
Li et al. (2005) investigated brain activation using fMRI during stress imagery in 10 women and 17 males who were abstinent for 2–3 weeks. There were no sex differences in stress-induced cocaine craving. Additionally, there were no sex differences in the observed negative correlations between stress-induced craving and activation of the anterior and posterior cingulate. However, sex differences (higher activation in women) were observed for stress-induced activation of left frontolimbic areas, including anterior cingulate, insula, dorsolateral and medial PFC, inferior frontal cortices, and posterior cingulate cortex.
B. Conclusions
The results of the studies reviewed demonstrate strong sex differences in the effect of cue- and stress-induced cocaine craving manipulations on brain activity, with both quantitative (different degrees of activation) and, unexpectedly, qualitative (opposite effects) differences (Fig. 6). These differential brain responses occurred despite the consistent lack of sex differences in cue- or stress-induced subjective craving. Thus, a tentative conclusion from these studies is that to the degree that correlational results from imaging studies reflect causes rather than consequences of drug craving, the brain circuits controlling cocaine craving in men and women are likely different. The studies reviewed also suggest that in both sexes the brain mechanisms of cue-induced versus stress-induced cocaine craving are largely dissociable. This conclusion is consistent with results from preclinical studies on differences in brain circuits of cue- versus stress-induced reinstatement of drug seeking (Shalev et al., 2002; Bossert et al., 2013; Reiner et al., 2019). Another observation from the studies reviewed is that cue-induced brain activation versus inhibition is dependent on the substance use disorder phase (active use vs. abstinence) (Kilts et al., 2004; Volkow et al., 2011).
Sex differences in cue- and stress-induced brain activation in humans who use cocaine. Schematic illustration of sex differences in (A) cocaine cue-induced and (B) stress-induced brain activation (assessed by PET or fMRI). F, female; M, male.
A limitation of the studies reviewed, particularly in reference to an important clinical outcome—relapse after prolonged abstinence—is that they were performed during early abstinence or ongoing cocaine use. Thus, the pattern of brain activation in both sexes in response to drug cues and stress during protracted abstinence is unknown. Finally, for the fMRI studies, a common problem in the studies reviewed was small sample sizes that are well below the recommended number of human participants required for reproducible task-based fMRI studies (Turner et al., 2018). Thus, future research should replicate these studies with larger sample sizes.
Finally, the implications of the sex differences in brain activity during cue- and stress-induced cocaine craving to the development of new medications are unknown, specifically in regard to the general conclusion of lack of sex differences in response to opioid agonist therapy. However, these sex differences in brain activity during cue- and stress-induced cocaine craving could be potentially used as brain markers to predict relapse in men and women.
VI. Conclusion and Clinical Implications
A prevailing notion in the preclinical addiction field is that females are more vulnerable than males to drug self-administration, withdrawal, and relapse across drug classes (Fattore et al., 2008; Becker, 2016; Carroll and Lynch, 2016). Here, we critically reviewed this notion with respect to psychostimulant and opioid craving and relapse in humans and rodent models. Unexpectedly, as our own research was guided by this notion (Zlebnik et al., 2014, 2021; Nicolas et al., 2019), our review does not support ubiquitous female susceptibility to craving and relapse.
Our main conclusion of the clinical literature is that the published studies do not support the idea that women are more vulnerable to psychostimulant and opioid craving and relapse. However, this conclusion is tentative because many of the studies reviewed were either correlational or not sufficiently powered to detect sex differences.
Our main conclusion of the preclinical literature is that there are sex differences in stress-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking and incubation of cocaine craving, which in the latter case are modulated in part by ovarian hormones. In contrast, there is minimal evidence for sex differences for either cue- or cocaine-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking. Additionally, the studies with methamphetamine and heroin (and other opioid drugs) do not support the notion of sex differences in reinstatement of drug seeking or incubation of craving for these drugs. However, for reinstatement of methamphetamine and heroin seeking, our conclusion is tentative because only a few studies were published, and the results are mixed.
The reasons for the drug-specific evidence of sex differences in rodent models are unknown. Potential reasons could be the distinct neurobiological mechanisms of relapse between opioids versus psychostimulants and cocaine versus methamphetamine (Badiani et al., 2011; Bossert et al., 2013) and the differential interaction of these drugs with ovarian hormones or organizational sex effects.
Another main conclusion is that fluctuations in ovarian hormones appear to play a role in cocaine craving in humans as well as cocaine-induced reinstatement and incubation of craving in rat models. The hypothesis that emerges from these studies is that progesterone decreases drug craving and relapse, whereas estradiol has an opposite effect (see Fig. 5 for a proposed brain mechanism for these effects).
An issue to consider from a translational perspective is the apparent lack of concordance between preclinical and clinical studies with cocaine, with some evidence supporting sex differences in rat models but not in humans. This discrepancy is not surprising because of the complex social, legal, cultural, and language-related factors that contribute to substance use disorders in humans that cannot be modeled in rodents (Heilig et al., 2016; de Wit et al., 2018; Venniro et al., 2020). However, a close inspection of the human results suggests some similarities, opportunities for future research, and potential treatment implications. Specifically, in some studies women report greater cocaine craving than men during early abstinence (Elman et al., 2001; Waldrop et al., 2010; Moran-Santa Maria et al., 2014). Additionally, several laboratory studies report decreased cocaine craving during the luteal versus follicular phase (Sofuoglu et al., 1999; Evans et al., 2002). Thus, abstinence attempts during the luteal phase may be more successful than those during the follicular phase. Indeed, similar approaches have led to favorable cessation outcomes in tobacco smokers (Allen et al., 2008, 2009a,b). Additionally, treatment with exogenous progesterone reduced cocaine craving (Sofuoglu et al., 2002, 2004; Evans and Foltin, 2006; Fox et al., 2013; Milivojevic et al., 2016). Together, these results suggest that in both humans and rodent models, the menstrual/estrous cycle contributes to cocaine seeking and highlights progesterone as a potential adjunct pharmacotherapy to reduce cocaine relapse.
The influence of gonadal hormones on drug craving is an example of how different mechanisms may differentially promote susceptibility to relapse in males and females. Additionally, as discussed above, despite the apparent lack of sex differences in self-reported craving and rates of relapse in humans, neuroimaging studies identified sex differences in regional brain activation associated with cue- and stress-induced cocaine craving (Fig. 6) (Kilts et al., 2004; Li et al., 2005; Volkow et al., 2011; Potenza et al., 2012; Zhang et al., 2020). These results suggest sex differences in brain mechanisms of cocaine (and potentially other drugs) craving and possibly relapse. This conclusion is supported by preclinical studies suggesting sex-specific mechanisms of cocaine-seeking behaviors (Calipari et al., 2017; Doncheck et al., 2020).
Variation in the underlying neurobiology of craving and relapse between men and women may result in differential treatment effects. For example, women experienced a greater reduction in stress- and cue-mediated cocaine craving than men after administration of the α2-adrenergic agonist guanfacine (Fox et al., 2014). Sex differences in “treatment” effects were also demonstrated for drug-seeking behavior in rodent models (Anker et al., 2009; Anker and Carroll, 2010b; Holtz et al., 2012; Zhou et al., 2012; Swalve et al., 2016; Witt and Reissner, 2020). For example, in a recent study Bossert et al. (2020) reported that female rats are less sensitive than males to the effect of the μ opioid receptor partial agonist TRV130 on context-induced reinstatement and reacquisition of oxycodone self-administration.
Finally, our review focused on degree and prevalence of self-reported craving and rates of relapse and found no clear evidence for sex differences. However, the etiology of these phenomena may differ between men and women (Back et al., 2011), and future studies should examine the complex factors that may differentially promote craving and relapse in both sexes.
Authorship Contributions
Wrote or contributed to the writing of the manuscript: Nicolas, Zlebnik, Farokhnia, Leggio, Ikemoto, Shaham.
Footnotes
- Received April 11, 2021.
- Accepted August 15, 2021.
The authors declare that they do not have any conflicts of interest (financial or otherwise) related to the text of the paper. The write-up of the review was supported by funds to National Institutes of Health National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA) Intramural Research Program (to Y.S., L.L., M.F., and S.I.), National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA) Division of Intramural Clinical and Biological Research (to L.L., M.F.), NIDA [Grant K99 DA047419] (to N.E.Z.), and Fondation pour la recherche médicale fellowship [Grant ARF201909009147] (to C.N.).
The authors declare that they do not have any competing interests or conflicts of interest (financial or otherwise) related to the material presented in this manuscript. An earlier version of this paper appears in MedRxiv: https://www.medrxiv.org/content/10.1101/2021.03.30.21254644v1.
↵1 C.N. and N.E.Z. contributed equally to this work.
↵
This article has supplemental material available at pharmrev.aspetjournals.org.
Abbreviations
- CRF
- corticotropin-releasing factor
- Drd2
- dopamine receptor 2
- fMRI
- functional magnetic resonance imaging
- NAc
- nucleus accumbens
- PAG
- periaqueductal gray
- PET
- positron emission tomography
- PFC
- prefrontal cortex
- vmPFC
- ventromedial prefrontal cortex
- VTA
- ventral tegmental area.
- U.S. Government work not protected by U.S. copyright