Elsevier

Biological Psychiatry

Volume 72, Issue 11, 1 December 2012, Pages 898-906
Biological Psychiatry

Priority Communication
Psilocybin Biases Facial Recognition, Goal-Directed Behavior, and Mood State Toward Positive Relative to Negative Emotions Through Different Serotonergic Subreceptors

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.04.005Get rights and content

Background

Serotonin (5-HT) 1A and 2A receptors have been associated with dysfunctional emotional processing biases in mood disorders. These receptors further predominantly mediate the subjective and behavioral effects of psilocybin and might be important for its recently suggested antidepressive effects. However, the effect of psilocybin on emotional processing biases and the specific contribution of 5-HT2A receptors across different emotional domains is unknown.

Methods

In a randomized, double-blind study, 17 healthy human subjects received on 4 separate days placebo, psilocybin (215 μg/kg), the preferential 5-HT2A antagonist ketanserin (50 mg), or psilocybin plus ketanserin. Mood states were assessed by self-report ratings, and behavioral and event-related potential measurements were used to quantify facial emotional recognition and goal-directed behavior toward emotional cues.

Results

Psilocybin enhanced positive mood and attenuated recognition of negative facial expression. Furthermore, psilocybin increased goal-directed behavior toward positive compared with negative cues, facilitated positive but inhibited negative sequential emotional effects, and valence-dependently attenuated the P300 component. Ketanserin alone had no effects but blocked the psilocybin-induced mood enhancement and decreased recognition of negative facial expression.

Conclusions

This study shows that psilocybin shifts the emotional bias across various psychological domains and that activation of 5-HT2A receptors is central in mood regulation and emotional face recognition in healthy subjects. These findings may not only have implications for the pathophysiology of dysfunctional emotional biases but may also provide a framework to delineate the mechanisms underlying psylocybin's putative antidepressant effects.

Section snippets

Subjects

Seventeen healthy, right-handed subjects (11 male subjects, 6 female subjects, mean age 26.0 ± 4.36 years, 15 university students/graduates, 1 high school diploma, 1 apprenticeship) were recruited through advertisement from the University of Zürich. All subjects were healthy according to physical examination, including electrocardiography and detailed blood analysis. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview for DSM-IV (38), the Expert System for Diagnosing Mental Disorders (39), and

PANAS

Repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) with pretreatment, treatment, and subscale as within-subject factors revealed that psilocybin significantly increased PANAS scores [F(1,16) = 16.608, p < .001, ηp2 = .509]. Importantly, the triple interaction between pretreatment × treatment × subscale [F(1,16) = 21.160, p < .001, ηp2 = .569] showed that psilocybin significantly increased positive affect subscale after pretreatment with placebo (p < .00001) but not ketanserin (p = 1) (Figure 1). In

Discussion

Our data show that psilocybin biases emotional processing toward positive relative to negative information, an effect that is consistent across different psychological domains. Specifically, psilocybin first enhanced positive mood states; second, decreased recognition of negative facial expression; and third, increased behavior toward positive relative to negative cues. In contrast to these generalized effects of the serotonergic agonist psilocybin, a more specific role for 5-HT2A receptors in

References (74)

  • E.S. da Silva et al.

    Facilitation of 5-HT(2A/2C)-mediated neurotransmission in the ventromedial hypothalamic nucleus decreases anxiety in the elevated T-maze

    Behav Brain Res

    (2011)
  • M. Kometer et al.

    The 5-HT2A/1A agonist psilocybin disrupts modal object completion associated with visual hallucinations

    Biol Psychiatry

    (2011)
  • D.E. Nichols

    Hallucinogens

    Pharmacol Ther

    (2004)
  • G. Hayward et al.

    Low-dose tryptophan depletion in recovered depressed patients induces changes in cognitive processing without depressive symptoms

    Biol Psychiatry

    (2005)
  • G. Bedi et al.

    Is ecstasy an “empathogen”?Effects of ±3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine on prosocial feelings and identification of emotional states in others

    Biol Psychiatry

    (2010)
  • O.J. Robinson et al.

    A double dissociation in the roles of serotonin and mood in healthy subjects

    Biol Psychiatry

    (2009)
  • A. Feder et al.

    Tryptophan depletion and emotional processing in healthy volunteers at high risk for depression

    Biol Psychiatry

    (2011)
  • S. Frühholz et al.

    Time course of implicit processing and explicit processing of emotional faces and emotional words

    Biol Psychol

    (2011)
  • F. Perrin et al.

    Spherical splines for scalp potential and current density mapping

    Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol

    (1989)
  • J.P. Roiser et al.

    The effects of tryptophan depletion on neural responses to emotional words in remitted depression

    Biol Psychiatry

    (2009)
  • A.R. Hariri et al.

    Genetics of emotional regulation: The role of the serotonin transporter in neural function

    Trends Cogn Sci

    (2006)
  • N.S. Burghardt et al.

    The selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor citalopram increases fear after acute treatment but reduces fear with chronic treatment: A comparison with tianeptine

    Biol Psychiatry

    (2004)
  • M. Yamauchi et al.

    Desensitization of 5-HT2A receptor function by chronic administration of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors

    Brain Res

    (2006)
  • K.P. Schulz et al.

    Does the emotional go/no-go task really measure behavioral inhibition? Convergence with measures on a non-emotional analog

    Arch Clin Neuropsychol

    (2007)
  • S.J. Thomas et al.

    Sequence effects in the Go/NoGo task: Inhibition and facilitation

    Int J Psychophysiol

    (2009)
  • E. Goeleven et al.

    Deficient inhibition of emotional information in depression

    J Affect Disord

    (2006)
  • P.H. Chiu et al.

    Dissociable recruitment of rostral anterior cingulate and inferior frontal cortex in emotional response inhibition

    Neuroimage

    (2008)
  • J.W. Krompinger et al.

    Electrophysiological indicators of emotion processing biases in depressed undergraduates

    Biol Psychol

    (2009)
  • J. Polich

    Updating p300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b

    Clin Neurophysiol

    (2007)
  • E. Nyman et al.

    Impact of temperament on depression and anxiety symptoms and depressive disorder in a population-based birth cohort

    J Affect Disord

    (2011)
  • J. Joormann et al.

    Is this happiness I see?Biases in the identification of emotional facial expressions in depression and social phobia

    J Abnorm Psychol

    (2006)
  • F.C. Murphy et al.

    Emotional bias and inhibitory control processes in mania and depression

    Psychol Med

    (1999)
  • K. Erickson et al.

    Mood-congruent bias in affective go/no-go performance of unmedicated patients with major depressive disorder

    Am J Psychiatry

    (2005)
  • R. Elliott et al.

    Affective cognition and its disruption in mood disorders

    Neuropsychopharmacology

    (2011)
  • L.N. Yatham et al.

    Decrease in brain serotonin 2 receptor binding in patients with major depression following desipramine treatment: A positron emission tomography study with fluorine-18-labeled setoperone

    Arch Gen Psychiatry

    (1999)
  • P.R. Albert et al.

    Modifying 5-HT1A receptor gene expression as a new target for antidepressant therapy

    Front Neurosci

    (2010)
  • J.H. Meyer et al.

    Dysfunctional attitudes and 5-HT2 receptors during depression and self-harm

    Am J Psychiatry

    (2003)
  • Cited by (205)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text