Exploratory, anxiety and spatial memory impairments are dissociated in mice lacking the LPA1 receptor

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2010.04.003Get rights and content

Abstract

Lysophosphatidic acid (LPA) is a new, intercellular signalling molecule in the brain that has an important role in adult hippocampal plasticity. Mice lacking the LPA1 receptor exhibit motor, emotional and cognitive alterations. However, the potential relationship among these concomitant impairments was unclear. Wild-type and maLPA1-null mice were tested on the hole-board for habituation and spatial learning. MaLPA1-null mice exhibited reduced exploration in a novel context and a defective intersession habituation that also revealed increased anxiety-like behaviour throughout the hole-board testing. In regard to spatial memory, maLPA1 nulls failed to reach the controls’ performance at the end of the reference memory task. Moreover, their defective working memory on the first training day suggested a delayed acquisition of the task’s working memory rule, which is also a long term memory component. The temporal interval between trials and the task’s difficulty may explain some of the deficits found in these mice. Principal components analysis revealed that alterations found in each behavioural dimension were independent. Therefore, exploratory and emotional impairments did not account for the cognitive deficits that may be attributed to maLPA1 nulls’ hippocampal malfunction.

Introduction

Lysophosphatidic acid (LPA, 1-acyl-2-sn-glycerol-3-phosphate), acting through 6 G protein-coupled receptors (LPA1–6), has gained increasing attention over the last few years as an intercellular messenger with several effects on different target tissues (Anliker and Chun, 2004, Birgbauer and Chun, 2006, Choi et al., 2010, Chun, 2005, Chun, 2007, Fukushima et al., 2001, Ishii et al., 2004, Moolenaar et al., 2004, Noguchi et al., 2009, Rivera and Chun, 2008). A growing body of evidence indicates that the LPA pathway is involved in normal and abnormal brain development and function (Anliker and Chun, 2004, Choi et al., 2008, Chun, 2005, Estivill-Torrus et al., 2008). The most extensively studied of these receptors is LPA1 (Chun, 2005, Contos et al., 2000, Estivill-Torrus et al., 2008, Fukushima et al., 2002, Herr and Chun, 2007, Kingsbury et al., 2003, Matas-Rico et al., 2008).

Recently, Santin et al. (2009) described the behavioural phenotype of the maLPA1-null mouse, a stable variant of the LPA1-null mutant strain formerly characterised by Contos et al. (2000) and described in Estivill-Torrús et al. (2008). Impaired spatial memory retention, abnormal use of searching strategies, altered exploration in the open field and increased anxiety-like responses in the elevated plus maze have been reported in the absence of retinal and auditory malfunctions. However, concomitant neurological deficits were observed in olfaction and somesthesis, limb reflexes, co-ordinated limb use and neuromuscular strength (Santin et al., 2009). Interestingly, these behavioural alterations are accompanied by impairments in both hippocampus and cerebral cortex that may be partially responsible for the phenotype (Estivill-Torrus et al., 2008, Matas-Rico et al., 2008).

The complexity of the behavioural phenotype exhibited by the maLPA1-null mice with impairments in several behavioural domains is frequently observed when transgenic mice are used in research (e.g. Acevedo et al., 2006, Kalueff et al., 2007, Santin et al., 2009). However, the potential relationship among sensorimotor, emotional and cognitive variables is not generally well-addressed and may lead to inaccurate interpretations. To date, it is known that anxiety-related behaviours, exploration and cognition may reflect dissociated or common processes in animal testing (Matzel et al., 2008, Miyagawa et al., 1998, Ohl et al., 2003, Ohl et al., 2002). In this regard, it has been reported that memory could be influenced by the rodent’s inborn anxiety or by its reactivity to a stressor (Herrero et al., 2006, Ribeiro et al., 1999, Wright et al., 2006). The relevance of this point is emphasized in reports that suggest that the performance of some mouse strains in certain tasks may reflect the strain’s anxiety-related behaviour, rather than cognitive functions (Dockstader and van der Kooy, 2001, Ohl et al., 2002). It is important to note that stressors, such as a novel environment or forced swimming, are usually an unavoidable part of the experimental setting even when studying non-emotional cognitive processes. Furthermore, the degree of aversion varies from one task to another, and that may explain disparate memory results between procedures (Hodges, 1996). On the other hand, anxiety levels could be related to increased or reduced locomotion (Kameda et al., 2007, Ramos and Mormede, 1998), and motor activity could influence anxiety and memory when their assessment involves spatial–temporal parameters (Brody and Holtzman, 2006, Kalueff et al., 2007, Strekalova et al., 2005).

The main purpose of this work is to study exploration, anxiety and spatial memory in maLPA1-null mice, with a focus on the interrelationship among these characteristics, to determine whether motor activity or anxiety impairments might account for cognitive performance. To address this issue, we used the hole-board test and the principal components analysis (PCA) multivariate approach. The hole-board is a frequently used hippocampal-dependent task for measuring spatial learning that is similar to the water maze in that extra-maze cues are used to solve the task (Oades, 1981). Moreover, the hole-board, as well as its modified version, allows the simultaneous evaluation of various potentially interrelated emotional, exploratory and spatial memory measures (Ohl et al., 2002, Ohl et al., 2003, Takeda et al., 1998). PCA is useful to resolve variables into the independent dimensions (factors) that underlie behaviour (Ohl et al., 2002, Ohl et al., 2003, Ramos and Mormede, 1998). Although PCA has successfully been applied to assess behavioural paradigms and inbred strains, it has been less frequently used in studies using transgenic animals (Carola et al., 2002, Fernandes et al., 1999, Gross et al., 2000, Ohl et al., 2003). In this study, we further show the utility of PCA in analysing behavioural research using mutant mice.

Section snippets

Animals

The generation and characterization of maLPA1-null mice have been previously described (Estivill-Torrus et al., 2008, Matas-Rico et al., 2008). The original-null mice were obtained by targeted gene disruption using homologous recombination and Cre-mediated deletion in a 129X1/SvJ background. These animals were then backcrossed with C57BL/6J mice. Intercrosses of these mice, as well as with mice generated from one additional backcross (Contos et al., 2000), were begun immediately. An LPA1-null

Response to novelty and habituation

To study the response to a novel context and habituation learning, 14 maLPA1-null and 23 analogous wild-type male mouse littermates were tested by hole-board test in 2 sessions of 3 min each carried out on 2 successive days, with all holes baited. Locomotion, percent of time in the maze’s periphery (time in periphery), rearing, head dipping, risk assessment, grooming and defecation were assessed.

Repeated measure ANOVAs carried out over the 2 habituation days revealed a significant ‘genotype’

Discussion

Our results provide reliable and robust evidence of the involvement of LPA through the LPA1 receptor signalling pathway in spatial memory in the absence of either emotional or motor influences. In agreement with a previous report (Santin et al., 2009), maLPA1-null mice exhibited a reduced exploratory behaviour when exposed to a novel environment (i.e., hole-board exploration during the first habituation session). Impaired exploration could be a consequence of the concomitant anxiety levels

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by grants from the Human Frontier Science Programme (J.C., F.R.D.F.), MEC SEJ2007-61187 (L.S.), I3SNS Programme (G.E.), FIS 02/1643, FIS PI07/0629 (G.E.), Red CIEN (G03/06) (F.R.D.F.), CTS065 and CTS433 (Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Spanish Ministry of Health and Andalusian Ministries of Health and of Innovation, Science and Enterprise) and the National Institutes of Health (USA) MH51699 and MH01723 (J.C.). The authors E. Castilla-Ortega and J. Sánchez-López were

References (64)

  • C. Gross et al.

    Altered fear circuits in 5-HT(1A) receptor KO mice

    Biological Psychiatry

    (2000)
  • S.M. Harrison et al.

    LPA1 receptor-deficient mice have phenotypic changes observed in psychiatric disease

    Molecular and Cellular Neuroscience

    (2003)
  • A.I. Herrero et al.

    Individual differences in anxiety trait are related to spatial learning abilities and hippocampal expression of mineralocorticoid receptors

    Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

    (2006)
  • H. Hodges

    Maze procedures: The radial-arm and water maze compared

    Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research

    (1996)
  • A. Holmes et al.

    Responses of Swiss–Webster mice to repeated plus-maze experience: Further evidence for a qualitative shift in emotional state?

    Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior

    (1998)
  • D. Joel et al.

    Effects of electrolytic lesions of the medial prefrontal cortex or its subfields on 4-arm baited, 8-arm radial maze, two-way active avoidance and conditioned fear tasks in the rat

    Brain Research

    (1997)
  • M.P. Leussis et al.

    Habituation in rodents: A review of behavior, neurobiology, and genetics

    Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2006)
  • E. Matas-Rico et al.

    Deletion of lysophosphatidic acid receptor LPA1 reduces neurogenesis in the mouse dentate gyrus

    Molecular and Cellular Neuroscience

    (2008)
  • H. Miyagawa et al.

    Dissociation of impairment between spatial memory, and motor function and emotional behavior in aged rats

    Behavioural Brain Research

    (1998)
  • K. Noguchi et al.

    Lysophosphatidic acid (LPA) and its receptors

    Current Opinion in Pharmacology

    (2009)
  • R.D. Oades

    Type of memory or attention? Impairments after lesions of the hippocampus and limbic ventral tegmentum

    Brain Research Bulletin

    (1981)
  • J. O’Keefe et al.

    The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat

    Brain Research

    (1971)
  • D.S. Olton et al.

    Spatial memory and hippocampal function

    Neuropsychologia

    (1979)
  • R.L. Ribeiro et al.

    The “anxiety state” and its relation with rat models of memory and habituation

    Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

    (1999)
  • P. Simon et al.

    Thigmotaxis as an index of anxiety in mice. Influence of dopaminergic transmissions

    Behavioural Brain Research

    (1994)
  • H. Takeda et al.

    Changes in head-dipping behavior in the hole-board test reflect the anxiogenic and/or anxiolytic state in mice

    European Journal of Pharmacology

    (1998)
  • C.M. Thiel et al.

    High versus low reactivity to a novel environment: Behavioural, pharmacological and neurochemical assessments

    Neuroscience

    (1999)
  • J. Allard et al.

    A rat G protein-coupled receptor selectively expressed in myelin-forming cells

    European Journal of Neuroscience

    (1998)
  • E. Birgbauer et al.

    New developments in the biological functions of lysophospholipids

    Cellular and Molecular Life Sciences

    (2006)
  • G.W. Bothe et al.

    Genetic and behavioral differences among five inbred mouse strains commonly used in the production of transgenic and knockout mice

    Genes, Brain & Behavior

    (2004)
  • J.W. Choi et al.

    LPA receptors: Subtypes and biological actions

    Annual Review of Pharmacology and Toxicology

    (2010)
  • J. Chun

    How the lysophospholipid got its receptor

    The Scientist

    (2007)
  • Cited by (68)

    • Biodistribution and toxicity assessment of copper nanoparticles in the rat brain

      2020, Journal of Trace Elements in Medicine and Biology
      Citation Excerpt :

      In addition, rearing of rats may reflect the cognition of rat to stressors such as the novelty of the environment during the test, in meaning that, in normal conditions when a rat is exposed to novel stimulus, it avoids it showing no exploratory trials and no rears. When time passes, the rat begins to approach and explore the stimulus that doesn't happen here with the treated rats during the time of experiment [66,67]. So, we think that the increase of copper content in most brain areas of the treated rats significantly affected their brain functions that altered their behaviours.

    • Training memory without aversion: Appetitive hole-board spatial learning increases adult hippocampal neurogenesis

      2018, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory
      Citation Excerpt :

      This study investigates AHN (immature neurons and cell proliferation) and hippocampal neuroplasticity markers related to AHN regulation [brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), insulin like growth factor-2 (IGF-II) and cAMP response element-binding protein (CREB) phosphorylation] in mice trained in an appetitively-motivated hole-board spatial navigation task. This paradigm allows training simultaneously of two different hippocampal-dependent cognitive components, spatial reference and working memory (Oades, 1981), and rewards are placed in four spatial locations which may increase the cognitive challenge compared to learning a single one (Castilla-Ortega et al., 2010; Sampedro-Piquero, Begega, & Arias, 2014). Although hole-board training modulates another aspects of hippocampal neuroplasticity such as long-term potentiation (Uzakov, Frey, & Korz, 2005), to date there is no information about the effect of hole-board training on AHN (Supplementary material).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text