Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Effects of Acute MDMA Intoxication on Mood and Impulsivity: Role of the 5-HT2 and 5-HT1 Receptors

  • Janelle H. P. van Wel ,

    Janelle.vanWel@maastrichtuniversity.nl

    Affiliation Department of Neuropsychology and Psychopharmacology, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, the Netherlands

  • Kim P. C. Kuypers,

    Affiliation Department of Neuropsychology and Psychopharmacology, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, the Netherlands

  • Eef L. Theunissen,

    Affiliation Department of Neuropsychology and Psychopharmacology, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, the Netherlands

  • Wendy M. Bosker,

    Affiliation Department of Neuropsychology and Psychopharmacology, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, the Netherlands

  • Katja Bakker,

    Affiliation Department of Neuropsychology and Psychopharmacology, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, the Netherlands

  • Johannes G. Ramaekers

    Affiliation Department of Neuropsychology and Psychopharmacology, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, the Netherlands

Abstract

MDMA induces positive mood and increases impulse control during intoxication, but only a few studies on the neuropharmacological mechanisms underlying these processes have been conducted. It was hypothesized that pretreatment with 5-HT1 and 5-HT2 receptor blockers would prevent MDMA effects on mood and impulsivity. Subjects (N = 17) participated in a double-blind, placebo controlled, within-subject design involving 6 experimental conditions consisting of pretreatment (T1) and treatment (T2). T1 preceded T2 by 30 minutes. T1–T2 combinations were: placebo-placebo, 20 mg pindolol-placebo, 50 mg ketanserin-placebo, placebo-75 mg MDMA, 20 mg pindolol-75 mg MDMA and 50 mg ketanserin-75 g MDMA. Subjects completed a Profile of Mood States (POMS) questionnaire and several impulsivity tasks (Stop signal task, Matching familiar figures task, Cue dependent reversal learning task) at 1.5 hrs post-treatment. MDMA alone increased both positive (vigor, arousal, friendliness, elation, positive mood) and negative affect (anxiety, confusion) as assessed by the POMS questionnaire. MDMA also increased stop reaction time in the Stop signal task and reaction time in the Matching familiar figures task. Pretreatment with ketanserin blocked MDMA effects on positive affect, but not negative affect. Ketanserin did not influence the effects of MDMA on impulsivity. Pindolol did not interact with MDMA on any of the measures. In conclusion, 5-HT2 receptors mediate positive moods induced by MDMA but not negative moods or impulsivity. 5-HT1 receptors do not appear to be involved in MDMA effects on mood and impulse control.

Trial Registration

Nederlands Trial Register NTR2352

Introduction

3,4-Methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) is a serotonin (5-HT) agonist and a reuptake inhibitor of serotonin and dopamine (DA) that has been shown to affect mood [1] and impulsivity during intoxication [2], [3], [4] and abstinence [5], [6]. Mood has been shown to be affected by fluctuations in 5-HT levels. High levels of 5-HT have been associated with mood elevations, while decrements in 5-HT are associated with depressed mood [7], [8], [9]. MDMA has been shown to increase 5-HT levels during intoxication but to deplete 5-HT after intake [8], [9], [10], [11]. Vollenweider and colleagues [4] discuss a series of studies in which a single dose of MDMA was shown to directly increase subjective feelings of openness, enhance mood and well-being and heighten feelings of happiness. Other researchers also reported that MDMA increased subjective ratings of feeling ‘loving’ and ‘friendly’, as indicated by the Profile of Mood States (POMS) questionnaire [12]. However, depressed mood has been reported in MDMA-users following acute administration of the drug, probably due to a depletion of 5-HT stores [13], [14], [15].

A similar relation may hold true for MDMA effects on impulsive behaviors. A reduction of 5-HT has been linked to impulsive, suicidal and aggressive behaviour [16] and substance abuse [17]. In contrast, high levels of 5-HT have been shown to decrease impulsive behaviour [16], [18]. Likewise, acute MDMA administration has been shown to increase impulse control, when 5-HT levels are high [1], [2], [3], [19], whereas impulse control decreased in the period following use, when 5-HT levels are low [6], [20], [21], [22], [23], [24], [25].

Besides general 5-HT levels, there is also evidence that 5-HT1 and 5-HT2 receptors contribute to the influence of MDMA on mood and impulsivity [26]. Liechti and Vollenweider [27] have shown that a 5-HT2 receptor antagonist (ketanserin) decreased the effects of MDMA on perception and emotional excitation. These investigators also demonstrated that a 5-HT1 blocker (pindolol) ameliorated some, but not all, of the MDMA-induced subjective effects [28]. The role of 5-HT1 and 5-HT2 antagonists on MDMA effects in impulsivity has not been studied before. However it has been shown that these receptors may play a role in impulse control. For example, 5-HT2 antagonists have been shown to decrease impulsivity [29] whereas 5-HT1 agonists have been associated with reductions in anxiety and impulsivity [30].

The present study was designed to assess the role of 5-HT1 and 5-HT2 receptors on the effects of MDMA on mood and impulse control. It was hypothesized that (1) an acute dose of MDMA would alter mood and increase impulse control; and that (2) the effects of MDMA on mood and impulsivity would be absent when pre-treated with 5-HT1 and 5-HT2 receptor blockers.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

Seventeen healthy MDMA-users (9 male, 8 female), aged between 19 and 27 (mean (SD) 22.76 (2.75)) years participated in the study. They were mild to moderate users of MDMA who reported to have taken the drug on 1 to 65 separate occasions (mean 10.94) in the previous year. Overall, subjects reported to have taken MDMA 3 to 780 occasions in their lifetime (mean 72.4 times). Subjects reported mean lifetime use of alcohol on 1168.5 occasions; cannabis on 554.4 occasions; amphetamines on 4.1 occasions; cocaine on 4.7 occasions; LSD on 4.4 occasions; mushrooms on 0.2 occasions; and other drugs, including but not limited to, khat, ketamine or salvia divinorum, 0.5 occasions.

Subjects were recruited through advertisements in local newspapers and by word of mouth. Before inclusion, subjects were examined by a medical supervisor, who checked vital signs and took blood samples for standard blood chemistry and haematology. Inclusion criteria were: written informed consent; age 18–35 yr; history of MDMA use; free from psychotropic medication; good physical health as assessed by a medical doctor; normal weight as determined by BMI 18–28. Exclusion criteria were: addiction according to DSM-IV criteria as assessed by a questionnaire; presence or history of psychiatric or neurological disorder as assessed during a clinical interview; pregnancy or lactating; cardiovascular abnormalities; excessive drinking or heavy smoking, i.e. defined as more than 20 standard units of alcohol per week and more than 10 cigarettes per day; hypertension. Subjects were given an information leaflet before the study explaining the entire study procedure. Subjects were fully aware of everything that would happen during the study, except the order of treatment.

This study was conducted according to the code of ethics on human experimentation established by the declaration of Helsinki (1964) and amended in Seoul (2008) and was approved by the Medical Ethics Committee of the Academic Hospital of Maastricht and Maastricht University. A permit for obtaining, storing and administering MDMA was obtained from the Dutch drug enforcement administration. Subjects were paid for their participation in the study.

Design, Doses and Administration

Subjects participated in a double-blind, placebo controlled, within-subject design involving 6 experimental conditions consisting of pretreatment (T1) and treatment (T2). T1 preceded T2 by 30 minutes. T1–T2 combinations were: placebo-placebo, 20 mg pindolol-placebo, 50 mg ketanserin-placebo, placebo-75 mg MDMA, pindolol 20 mg-MDMA 75 mg and 50 mg ketanserin-75 mg MDMA. Conditions were separated by minimum wash-out period of 7 days to avoid cross-condition contamination. Order of conditions was divided in 3 blocks of 6 conditions. The 75 mg dose of (racemic) MDMA was selected because it falls within the normal range of recreational use [24] and has been consistently shown to impair performance and produce robust subjective mood changes in a number of previous studies from our group [31], [32], [33]. Doses of pindolol 20 mg and ketanserin 50 mg represent regular therapeutic doses that block approximately 40% of 5-HT1A receptors and 91% of 5-HT2 receptors respectively [34], [35], [36]. MDMA, pindolol and ketanserin were acquired through the local hospital pharmacy, which also performed randomization, capsulation and distribution of study drugs.

Procedures

All subjects received a training session before onset of the experimental sessions in order to familiarize them with the tests and procedures. Subjects were asked to refrain from drugs at least a week before the start of the experiment and during the study. Subjects were not allowed to use alcohol on the day prior to an experimental session and were requested to arrive at experimental sessions well rested. Drug and alcohol screens were performed upon arrival of the subject. Drug screens assessed the presence of benzodiazepines, opiates, cocaine, marijuana, MDMA and (meth)amphetamine. A pregnancy test was performed for the female subjects. Study treatments were only administered if subjects tested negative for drugs, alcohol and pregnancy.

Treatments at T1 and T2 were administered using a double dummy technique to synchronize time of maximal drug concentrations (Tmax) and were administered as identical encapsulated tablets to ensure blinding. Mood and impulsivity were assessed by means of a number of tasks between 1.5–2 hrs after T2 (at Tmax). In between, subjects were allowed to read a book or watch television. In addition, blood pressure and body temperature were assessed as safety measures (more details about physiological measures are described in a previous paper by van Wel et al. [37]). A schematic representation of a testing day is shown in figure 1.

Subjective Measures

Profile of Mood States.

The Profile of Mood States (POMS) is a self-assessment mood questionnaire with 72 items, rated on a 5-point Likert scale, with 0 being ‘not at all’ to 4 ‘extremely’. Subjects had to indicate to what extent these items were representative of their mood at that moment in time. Eight mood states are classified and quantified by calculating the sum score of associated items for each mood state, i.e., anxiety (9 items), depression (15 items), anger (12 items), vigor (8 items), fatigue (7 items), confusion (7 items), friendliness (8 items) and elation (6 items). Two composite scales were derived; arousal ((anxiety+vigor)−(fatigue+confusion)) and positive mood (elation−depression) [38].

Impulsivity Tests

Matching Familiar Figures Task.

The computerized version of the Matching Familiar Figures Test (MFF20) was derived from the original Matching Familiar Figures Test [39] by Cairns and Cammock [40] and serves as a measure of reflection impulsivity. This is the tendency to reflect on the validity of a problem to be solved under the specific condition that there are more alternatives available while there is some uncertainty over which is the right choice. Thus, subjects who tend to respond more impulsively are faster, but make more errors, while those who pause and think about the alternatives are slower and more accurate. This task was developed to assess the processes involved in the gathering and evaluation of perceptual information required to make a response. The MFF20 seems to share some variability with other impulsivity tasks, such as the stop-signal task, but not with tasks involving reward [41].

The test format of the computerized MFF20 involves simultaneous presentation of a target figure positioned on the left of the screen and an array of six alternatives on the right half of the screen, all except one differing in one or more details from the target figure. The subjects are asked to select from the alternatives the figure that exactly matches the target as quickly as possible. This is achieved by pressing the number corresponding to the figure on a computer keyboard. If the initial selection is incorrect, this is signaled with a beep and subjects are required to give another answer. Each subject is given 2 practice trials followed by 20 test trials.

Two dependent measures, mean latency to first response and total number of errors, are automatically recorded. Two additional dependent variables can be calculated: an Impulsivity score (I-score) and an Efficiency score (E-score). The I-score is a composite index of impulsivity [42], [43] and is calculated by subtracting the standard score of the mean latency to first response from the standard score of the total number of errors committed (Zerror-Zlatency). The E-score is calculated by summing the standard score of the mean latency to first response with the standard score of the total number of errors committed (1-(Zerror+Zlatency)).

Stop signal task.

The stop signal task provides a measure of motor impulsivity. It requires subjects to make quick responses to visually presented go signals and to inhibit their response when a visual stop signal is suddenly presented. The current test is adapted from an earlier version of Fillmore and colleagues [44] and has been shown to be sensitive to stimulating as well as sedating drugs [3]. The go signals are four 1.5 cm letters (ABCD) presented one at a time in the center of a computer screen. Subjects are required to respond to each letter as quickly as possible by pressing one of two response buttons. One button is pressed to indicate that “A” or “C” appeared and the other to indicate “B” or “D”. Letters are displayed for 500 milliseconds and the computer screen is blank for 1.5 seconds before the next letter is displayed. This provides a period of 2 seconds in which the subject can respond to a letter. A single test consists of 176 trials in which each of the 4 stimuli is presented equally often. A stop signal occurs in 48 trials during the test. The stop signal consists of a visual cue, i.e. “*”, that appears in one of the four corners of the screen. Subjects are required to withhold their response in case a stop signal is presented. Stop signals are presented 12 times at each of the four delays after the onset of a letter: 50, 150, 250 and 350 milliseconds. Trials always begin with a 500 millisecond preparation interval in which a fixation cross appears at the center of the screen. Dependent variables are the proportion of commission errors on stop signal trials and the reaction times on go and stop signal trials (i.e. stop reaction time). Stop reaction time (SRT) to stop signal trials represents the estimated mean time required to inhibit a response.

The method for calculating stop reaction time was taken from the race model of inhibitory control [45]. This model proposes that the response to stop signal trials is defined by two parallel processes: execution of a motor action in response to a signal and inhibition of a motor action in response to a stop signal. Crucial to the outcome of the race is the speed of both processes. Response inhibition will fail if the time required to inhibit exceeds the time to complete a motor response at the time of the stop signal. The speed of the inhibition response cannot be observed directly but can be derived mathematically on the basis of three factors: stop-signal delay, reaction time distribution on go trials and the probability of successful response inhibitions in stop signal trials. First, reaction times to 128 go trials were rank ordered from shortest to longest. The finishing time of the inhibition response was then determined from the probability of successful response inhibition and the distribution of reaction times. If n-percent of the responses on stop-signal trials would be unsuccessfully inhibited (i.e. commission error), than the finishing time would be associated with the n-th percentile of the RT distribution. Stop reaction time was then determined by subtracting the appropriate stop-signal delay from reaction time at the n-th percentile of the RT distribution. The resulting values for each stop signal delay were then averaged to yield a single measure of stop reaction time for the test [3].

Cue-dependent reversal learning task.

The cue-dependent reversal learning task is an adjusted form of the cue-dependent go-no-go task of Fillmore and Rush [46]. This test places emphasis on the anticipatory nature of inhibitory and activational mechanisms of control, which rapidly develop cue-dependence. It has also been shown to be highly sensitive to the effects of psychoactive drugs [47]. In this task, subjects are required to respond to target stimuli (Go) and to inhibit their response on non-target stimuli (No-Go). Stimuli are rectangles, appearing in the center of a computer screen, in a horizontal or vertical position. Targets are green and non-targets are blue rectangles. Cues provide preliminary information regarding the type of imperative target stimulus (i.e. Go or No-Go) that is likely to follow. The cues have a high probability of signaling the correct target. The ‘vertical rectangle’ precedes a Go-stimulus in 80% of the cases and a No-Go-stimulus in 20% of the cases. The ‘horizontal rectangle’ signals a No-Go in 80% of the cases and a Go-stimulus in 20% of the cases. This rule will be reversed a number of times throughout this task, depending on the performance of the subject. Subjects have to detect this rule-change and modify their response pattern. Subjects will be informed beforehand that the rule can change but not about the number of times this event will take place or when it is changed. This task measures the acquisition and discrimination-reversal learning of cue-target associations. More impulsive individuals have more problems inhibiting responses after a reversal of the rule than less impulsive individuals [47]. Dependent variables are number of correct responses and correct inhibitions.

thumbnail
Table 1. Mean (SE) values of summated scales on the POMS questionnaire, followed by a summary of main effects and interactions following 2 major GLM analyses.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0040187.t001

thumbnail
Figure 2. Mean (SE) ratings on the Profile of Mood States (POMS) questionnaire in every treatment condition (n = 17).

ANX  =  anxiety, DEP  =  depression, ANG  =  anger, VIG  =  vigor, FTG  =  fatigue, CON  =  confusion, FRD  =  friendliness, ELT  =  elation, ASL  =  arousal, POS  =  positive mood.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0040187.g002

Blood Samples

Blood samples were collected before the start of the tasks at 1.5 hrs post T2. Blood samples were centrifuged immediately and the serum was subsequently frozen at −20°C until analyses for pharmacokinetic assessments. MDMA, pindolol and ketanserin concentrations were determined using solid phase extraction and gas chromatography with mass spectrometric detection.

Statistics

The hypothesis that pretreatment with ketanserin or pindolol would interact with MDMA induced impulsivity and mood was tested in 2 separate General Linear Model (GLM) analyses. Impulsivity and mood effects of MDMA, Ketanserin and MDMA × Ketanserin were analyzed by means of a GLM repeated measures ANOVA with MDMA (2 levels, i.e. 75 mg MDMA and placebo) and Ketanserin (50 mg ketanserin and 50 mg ketanserin +75 mg MDMA) as the main factors. Impulsivity and mood effects of MDMA, Pindolol and MDMA × Pindolol were analyzed by means of a GLM repeated measures ANOVA with MDMA (2 levels, i.e. 75 mg MDMA and placebo) and Pindolol (20 mg pindolol and 20 mg pindolol +75 mg MDMA) as the main factors. In case of significant main effects, separate drug-placebo contrasts were conducted. The alpha criterion significance level was set at p = 0.05. All statistical tests were conducted with SPSS version 15.0.

thumbnail
Table 2. Mean (SE) scores and Summary of main effects and interactions following 2 major GLM analyses for all dependent variables in the matching familiar figures task (MFF20), the stop signal task (SST) and the cue-dependent reversal learning task.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0040187.t002

Results

Seventeen complete data sets entered statistical analysis, except for the Stop signal task (N = 14), where 3 subjects were excluded due to technical or performance failures during test administration.

Subjective Measures

POMS.

A summary of GLM statistics for all POMS scales is given in Table 1. Mean (SE) subjective ratings on the POMS scales in every treatment condition are shown in Figure 2.

MDMA significantly increased feelings of anxiety, confusion, vigor, friendliness, elation, positive mood and arousal. The effect of MDMA on positive mood was highly significant (p<.001) in the GLM model including pindolol, and almost reached significance (p = 0.057) in the GLM model including ketanserin. In addition, MDMA also significantly reduced feelings of fatigue. Pindolol significantly increased feelings of confusion and decreased positive mood. There was no significant interaction between pindolol and MDMA. Ketanserin affected almost all subscales of the POMS. It significantly increased feelings of depression, fatigue and confusion and decreased feelings of vigor, friendliness, elation, arousal and positive mood. The interaction between MDMA and ketanserin reached significance on 4 POMS subscales: depression, friendliness, elation and positive mood. After the combination of MDMA and ketanserin, ratings of friendliness, elation and positive mood returned to placebo levels, whereas feelings of depression increased.

Impulsivity Tasks

The mean (SE) performance on impulsivity tasks for every treatment condition and a summary of GLM statistics for all impulsivity parameters is given in Table 2.

MDMA significantly increased SRT in the stop signal task and RT in the MFF20 but generally failed to affect performance on any of the other impulsivity parameters. Pindolol did not affect any impulsivity parameters. Ketanserin increased SRT and RT in the stop signal task and decreased the number of inhibitions in the cue-dependent reversal learning task. There were no significant interactions between MDMA and ketanserin. There were also no interactions between MDMA and pindolol other than an increase in correct inhibitions in the cue-dependent reversal learning task.

Blood Samples

Mean (SD) MDMA concentration in serum at 1.5 hours after MDMA administration was 157 (48) ng/mL. When combined with ketanserin or pindolol, mean (SD) serum concentration was 164 (62) and 156 (56) ng/mL respectively. Mean (SD) serum concentrations of ketanserin and pindolol were 86 (42) and 133 (80) ng/mL respectively. In the condition where ketanserin or pindolol was combined with MDMA, the mean (SD) serum concentrations of both drugs were 104 (41) and 130 (53) ng/mL respectively.

Discussion

The aim of the current study was to investigate the role of 5-HT1 and 5-HT2 receptors in MDMA induced changes in mood and impulsive behavior. Single doses of MDMA significantly increased positive as well as negative moods as rated with the POMS questionnaire. MDMA raised feelings of vigour, friendliness, elation and arousal, while also making subjects feel more anxious and confused. These findings are in line with previous studies that also reported a marked effect, both positive and negative, of MDMA administration on mood ratings [1], [2], [12], [19], [26], [48], [49]. Single doses of MDMA increased SRT in a stop signal task and reaction time in the MFF20 indicating a slowing of inhibitory and reflective responses during these tasks. Other measures of impulsivity did not show any effects of MDMA. In general however, acute effects of MDMA on mood and impulsivity were sufficiently present to assess the contributing roles of 5-HT1 and 5-HT2 receptors during pretreatments with ketanserin and pindolol.

Pretreatment with ketanserin significantly interacted with MDMA on the subscales representing positive moods (friendliness, elation and positive mood). Blockade of 5-HT2 receptors with ketanserin basically prevented MDMA to affect positive moods at all. POMS ratings of positive mood during the combination of ketanserin and MDMA were similar to the ratings during placebo. Ketanserin alone also significantly decreased positive mood rating. The magnitude of these effects was very small relative to the increase in positive moods produced by MDMA. Consequently, combined effects of ketanserin and MDMA cannot be explained as a summation of drug effects produced by MDMA and ketanserin separately, but truly indicates a drug interaction indicating that blockade of 5-HT2 receptors also blocks MDMA effects on positive moods. Pretreatment with ketanserin however did not reverse MDMA induced anxiety and the combination increased ratings of depression. This finding strongly indicates that the 5-HT2 receptor is only involved in mediating positive moods during intoxication and is not involved in some of the negative moods produced by MDMA.

Pretreatment with pindolol did not interact with the effects of MDMA on mood. When given alone, pindolol produced small but significant increments in feelings of confusion and small decrements in positive mood. The lack of interaction between MDMA and pindolol illustrated that the 5-HT1 receptor does not play a role in mediating MDMA induced mood states. The finding is in line with a previous mechanistic study [50] showing that pretreatment with pindolol does not affect MDMA induced moods. Alternatively, one could also argue that pindolol blocks only 40% of 5HT1A receptors [34] and that this may not suffice to measurably attenuate any 5HT1A mediated MDMA effects. We cannot exclude this possibility, but unfortunately, alternative 5HT1A ligands that fully block 5HT1A receptors are presently not available.

None of the pretreatments interacted with the effects of MDMA on measures of impulsivity. It should be noted, however, that the effects of MDMA were limited to an increment of SRT in the stop signal task and reaction time in the MFF20 and did not affect other measures of impulsivity. Previous studies have also shown a mix of either positive or neutral effects of single doses of MDMA on impulsivity [1], [2], [16], [18], [19]. Possibly, the lack of MDMA effects on most measures of impulsivity may be related to the fact that these measures represent different psychological and neuropharmacological constructs of impulse control [51]. Impulsivity is not a unitary, one-dimensional construct but can encompass different types of impulsivity [52], [53], [54]. Two types of impulsivity that can be distinguished are cognitive impulsivity and motor impulsivity. Cognitive impulsivity, as measured by the MFF20, is believed to reflect complex processes involved in the control of several cognitive, behavioral and effective processes. Motor impulsivity or response inhibition as measured by the stop signal task and the cue dependent reversal learning task, on the other hand, is believed to relate to the executive control of motor processes only [55]. Thus, it is possible that MDMA affects only a subset of processes related to motor impulsivity and cognitive impulsivity but leaves other subsets unaffected [3]. Alternatively, the relationship between MDMA and impulsivity may also be marginal and not a key feature during MDMA intoxication.

In conclusion, results from the current study show that administration of MDMA has both positive and negative influences on mood states. Furthermore, pretreatment with a 5-HT2 receptor antagonist affects MDMA-mediated responses on a number of positive subscales of the POMS, suggesting that the 5-HT2 receptor might be involved in mediating positive mood states. On the contrary, treatment with a partial 5-HT1 receptor antagonist did not interfere with MDMA effects on mood. Blockade of 5-HT1 and 5-HT2 receptors did not interact with the effects of MDMA on measures of impulse control.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Dr S Toennes and Dr G Kauert (Institute of Forensic Toxicology, Goethe University Frankfurt) for establishing drug-serum concentrations and MSc Uschi Poesmans for her assistance in data collection.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: JR. Performed the experiments: JvW KB WB. Analyzed the data: JvW KK ET JR. Wrote the paper: JvW. Extensive proof-reading of manuscript: ET KK JR.

References

  1. 1. Bosker WM, Kuypers KP, Conen S, Ramaekers JG (2010) Dose-related effects of MDMA on psychomotor function and mood before, during, and after a night of sleep loss. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 209: 69–76.
  2. 2. Kuypers KPC, Wingen M, Samyn N, Limbert N, Ramaekers JG (2007) Acute effects of nocturnal doses of MDMA on measures of impulsivity and psychomotor performance throughout the night. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 192: 111–119.
  3. 3. Ramaekers JG, Kuypers KPC (2006) Acute effects of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) on behavioral measures of impulsivity: alone and in combination with alcohol. Neuropsychopharmacology 31: 1048–1055.
  4. 4. Vollenweider FX, Liechti ME, Gamma A, Greer G, Geyer M (2002) Acute psychological and neurophysiological effects of MDMA in humans. J Psychoactive Drugs 34: 171–184.
  5. 5. Curran HV, Travill RA (1997) Mood and cognitive effects of +/−3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA, ‘ecstasy’): week-end ‘high’ followed by mid-week low. Addiction 92: 821–831.
  6. 6. Morgan MJ (1998) Recreational use of “ecstasy” (MDMA) is associated with elevated impulsivity. Neuropsychopharmacology 19: 252–264.
  7. 7. Young SN, Leyton M (2002) The role of serotonin in human mood and social interaction. Insight from altered tryptophan levels. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 71: 857–865.
  8. 8. Flory JD, Manuck SB, Matthews KA, Muldoon MF (2004) Serotonergic function in the central nervous system is associated with daily ratings of positive mood. Psychiatry Res 129: 11–19.
  9. 9. Toker L, Amar S, Bersudsky Y, Benjamin J, Klein E (2010) The biology of tryptophan depletion and mood disorders. Isr J Psychiatry Relat Sci 47: 46–55.
  10. 10. Schmidt CJ (1987) Neurotoxicity of the Psychedelic Amphetamine, Methylenedioxymethamphetamine. The Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics 240: 1–7.
  11. 11. Parrott AC (2012) MDMA and serotonergic neurotoxicity: the empirical evidence for its adverse effects in humans-no need for translation. Br J Pharmacol.
  12. 12. Bedi G, Hyman D, de Wit H (2010) Is Ecstasy an “Empathogen”? Effects of +/−3,4-Methylenedioxymethamphetamine on Prosocial Feelings and Identification of Emotional States in Others. Biol Psychiatry.
  13. 13. Parrott AC, Lasky J (1998) Ecstasy (MDMA) effects upon mood and cognition: before, during and after a Saturday night dance. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 139: 261–268.
  14. 14. Parrott AC, Lock J, Conner AC, Kissling C, Thome J (2008) Dance clubbing on MDMA and during abstinence from Ecstasy/MDMA: prospective neuroendocrine and psychobiological changes. Neuropsychobiology 57: 165–180.
  15. 15. Verheyden SL, Hadfield J, Calin T, Curran HV (2002) Sub-acute effects of MDMA (+/−3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine, “ecstasy”) on mood: evidence of gender differences. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 161: 23–31.
  16. 16. Fitzgerald PJ (2011) A neurochemical yin and yang: does serotonin activate and norepinephrine deactivate the prefrontal cortex? Psychopharmacology (Berl) 213: 171–182.
  17. 17. Kirby LG, Zeeb FD, Winstanley CA (2011) Contributions of serotonin in addiction vulnerability. Neuropharmacology.
  18. 18. Pattij T, Vanderschuren LJ (2008) The neuropharmacology of impulsive behaviour. Trends Pharmacol Sci 29: 192–199.
  19. 19. Kuypers KPC, Samyn N, Ramaekers JG (2006) MDMA and alcohol effects, combined and alone, on objective and subjective measures of actual driving performance and psychomotor function. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 187: 467–475.
  20. 20. Butler GK, Montgomery AM (2004) Impulsivity, risk taking and recreational ‘ecstasy’ (MDMA) use. Drug Alcohol Depend 76: 55–62.
  21. 21. Morgan MJ, Impallomeni LC, Pirona A, Rogers RD (2006) Elevated impulsivity and impaired decision-making in abstinent Ecstasy (MDMA) users compared to polydrug and drug-naive controls. Neuropsychopharmacology 31: 1562–1573.
  22. 22. Piechatzek M, Indlekofer F, Daamen M, Glasmacher C, Lieb R, et al. (2009) Is moderate substance use associated with altered executive functioning in a population-based sample of young adults? Hum Psychopharmacol 24: 650–665.
  23. 23. Quednow BB, Kuhn KU, Hoppe C, Westheide J, Maier W, et al. (2007) Elevated impulsivity and impaired decision-making cognition in heavy users of MDMA (“Ecstasy”). Psychopharmacology (Berl) 189: 517–530.
  24. 24. Gouzoulis-Mayfrank E, Daumann J (2009) Neurotoxicity of drugs of abuse–the case of methylenedioxyamphetamines (MDMA, ecstasy), and amphetamines. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 11: 305–317.
  25. 25. Reneman L, Endert E, de Bruin K, Lavalaye J, Feenstra MG, et al. (2002) The acute and chronic effects of MDMA (“ecstasy”) on cortical 5-HT2A receptors in rat and human brain. Neuropsychopharmacology 26: 387–396.
  26. 26. Liechti ME, Saur MR, Gamma A, Hell D, Vollenweider FX (2000) Psychological and physiological effects of MDMA (“ecstasy”) after pretreatment with the 5-HT2 antagonist Ketanserin in healthy humans. Neuropsychopharmacology 23: 396–404.
  27. 27. Liechti ME, Vollenweider FX (2001) Which neuroreceptors mediate the subjective effects of MDMA in humans? A summary of mechanistic studies. Hum Psychopharmacol 16: 589–598.
  28. 28. Vollenweider FX, Studerus E, Hasler F, Paulus M (2006) Role of 5-HT1A receptors in the acute effects of MDMA on cognition and emotion in humans. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology 9: S73–S73.
  29. 29. Anastasio NC, Stoffel EC, Fox RG, Bubar MJ, Rice KC, et al. (2011) Serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine) 5-HT2A receptor: association with inherent and cocaine-evoked behavioral disinhibition in rats. Behav Pharmacol 22: 248–261.
  30. 30. Schreiber R, De Vry J (1993) 5-HT1A receptor ligands in animal models of anxiety, impulsivity and depression: multiple mechanisms of action? Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 17: 87–104.
  31. 31. Kuypers KPC, Ramaekers JG (2005) Transient memory impairment after acute dose of 75 mg 3.4-Methylene-dioxymethamphetamine. Journal of Psychopharmacology 19: 633–639.
  32. 32. Kuypers KPC, Ramaekers JG (2007) Acute dose of MDMA (75 mg) impairs spatial memory for location but leaves contextual processing of visuospatial information unaffected. Psychopharmacology 189: 557–563.
  33. 33. Ramaekers JG, Kuypers KP, Wingen M, Heinecke A, Formisano E (2009) Involvement of inferior parietal lobules in prospective memory impairment during acute MDMA (ecstasy) intoxication: an event-related fMRI study. Neuropsychopharmacology 34.
  34. 34. Brogden RN, Sorkin EM (1990) Ketanserin. A review of its pharmacodynamic and pharmacokinetic properties, and therapeutic potential in hypertension and peripheral vascular disease. Drugs 40: 903–949.
  35. 35. Rabiner EA, Gunn RN, Wilkins MR, Sargent PA, Mocaer E, et al. (2000) Drug action at the 5-HT(1A) receptor in vivo: autoreceptor and postsynaptic receptor occupancy examined with PET and [carbonyl-(11)C]WAY-100635. Nucl Med Biol 27: 509–513.
  36. 36. Sharpley AL, Elliott JM, Attenburrow MJ, Cowen PJ (1994) Slow wave sleep in humans: role of 5-HT2A and 5-HT2C receptors. Neuropharmacology 33: 467–471.
  37. 37. van Wel JH, Kuypers KP, Theunissen EL, Bosker WM, Bakker K, et al. (2011) Blockade of 5-HT(2) Receptor Selectively Prevents MDMA-Induced Verbal Memory Impairment. Neuropsychopharmacology 36: 1932–1939.
  38. 38. de Wit H, Enggasser JL, Richards JB (2002) Acute administration of d-amphetamine decreases impulsivity in healthy volunteers. Neuropsychopharmacology 27: 813–825.
  39. 39. Kagan J (1966) Reflection–impulsivity: the generality and dynamics of conceptual tempo. J Abnorm Psychol 71: 17–24.
  40. 40. Cairns E, Cammock T (1978) Development of a more reliable version of the Matching Familiar Figures Test. Developmental Psychology 14: 555–560.
  41. 41. Perales JC, Verdejo-Garcia A, Moya M, Lozano O, Perez-Garcia M (2009) Bright and dark sides of impulsivity: performance of women with high and low trait impulsivity on neuropsychological tasks. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 31: 927–944.
  42. 42. Messer SB, Brodzinsky DM (1981) Three year stability of reflection-impulsivity in young adolescents. Developmental Psychology 17: 848–850.
  43. 43. Salkind NJ, Wright JC (1977) The development of reflection impulsivity and cognitive efficiency: An integrated model. Human Development 20: 377–387.
  44. 44. Fillmore MT, Rush CR, Hays L (2002) Acute effects of oral cocaine on inhibitory control of behavior in humans. Drug Alcohol Depend 67: 157–167.
  45. 45. Logan GD (1994) On the ability to inhibit though and action: a users’ guide to the stop signal paradigm. In: Dagenbach D, Carr TH, editors. pp. 189–239. pp.
  46. 46. Fillmore MT, Rush CR (2006) Polydrug abusers display impaired discrimination-reversal learning in a model of behavioural control. J Psychopharmacol 20: 24–32.
  47. 47. Fillmore MT, Rush CR, Hays L (2006) Acute effects of cocaine in two models of inhibitory control: implications of non-linear dose effects. Addiction 101: 1323–1332.
  48. 48. Liechti ME, Baumann C, Gamma A, Vollenweider FX (2000) Acute psychological effects of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA, “Ecstasy”) are attenuated by the serotonin uptake inhibitor citalopram. Neuropsychopharmacology 22: 513–521.
  49. 49. Parrott AC, Stuart M (1997) Ecstasy (MDMA), amphetamine, and LSD: Comparative mood profiles in recreational polydrug users. Human Psychopharmacology-Clinical and Experimental 12: 501–504.
  50. 50. Hasler F, Studerus E, Lindner K, Ludewig S, Vollenweider FX (2009) Investigation of serotonin-1A receptor function in the human psychopharmacology of MDMA. Journal of Psychopharmacology 23: 923–935.
  51. 51. Talpos JC, Wilkinson LS, Robbins TW (2006) A comparison of multiple 5-HT receptors in two tasks measuring impulsivity. J Psychopharmacol 20: 47–58.
  52. 52. Chambers CD, Garavan H, Bellgrove MA (2009) Insights into the neural basis of response inhibition from cognitive and clinical neuroscience. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 33: 631–646.
  53. 53. Dalley JW, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW (2011) Impulsivity, compulsivity, and top-down cognitive control. Neuron 69: 680–694.
  54. 54. Evenden JL (1999) Varieties of impulsivity. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 146: 348–361.
  55. 55. Friedman NP, Miyake A (2004) The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: a latent-variable analysis. J Exp Psychol Gen 133: 101–135.